Orientation specificity in spatial memory: what makes a path different from a map of the path?

Three studies investigated the factors that lead spatial information to be stored in an orientation-specific versus orientation-free manner. In Experiment 1, we replicated the findings of Presson and Hazelrigg (1984) that learning paths from a small map versus learning the paths directly from viewing a world leads to different functional characteristics of spatial memory. Whether the route display was presented as the path itself or as a large map of the path did not affect how the information was stored. In Experiment 2, we examined the effects of size of stimulus display, size of world, and scale transformations on how spatial information in maps is stored and available for use in later judgments. In Experiment 3, we examined the effect of size on the orientation specificity of the spatial coding of paths that are viewed directly. The major determinant of whether spatial information was stored and used in an orientation-specific or an orientation-free manner was the size of the display. Small displays were coded in an orientation-specific way, whereas very large displays were coded in a more orientation-free manner. These data support the view that there are distinct spatial representations, one more perceptual and episodic and one more integrated and model-like, that have developed to meet different demands faced by mobile organisms.

[1]  A. W. Melton,et al.  Coding Processes in Human Memory. , 1973 .

[2]  Irvin Rock,et al.  Orientation and form , 1974 .

[3]  W. H. Ittelson Environment and cognition , 1974 .

[4]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[5]  G W Evans,et al.  Cognitive mapping: knowledge of real-world distance and location information. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[6]  Irwin N. Jankovic,et al.  Principles of spatial problem solving. , 1982 .

[7]  Barbara Hayes-Roth,et al.  Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[8]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Coordinate systems in the long-term memory representation of three-dimensional shapes , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[9]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Building spatial representations through primary and secondary learning. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  E. Strelow What is needed for a theory of mobility: direct perception and cognitive maps--lessons from the blind. , 1985, Psychological review.

[11]  On the varieties of spatial knowledge: Cognitive maps and imagined terrains. , 1985 .

[12]  M. Just,et al.  Cognitive coordinate systems: accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. , 1985, Psychological review.

[13]  J. Bartlett,et al.  Cognitive impenetrability of memory for orientation. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Evolution of Multiple Memory Systems , 1987 .

[15]  N. Eisenberg Contemporary topics in developmental psychology , 1987 .

[16]  Clark C. Presson,et al.  Orientation-specificity in kinesthetic spatial learning: The role of multiple orientations , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[17]  Sholl Mj Cognitive maps as orienting schemata. , 1987 .