Predicting one's own forgetting: the role of experience-based and theory-based processes.

The authors examined the hypothesis that judgments of learning (JOL), if governed by processing fluency during encoding, should be insensitive to the anticipated retention interval. Indeed, neither item-by-item nor aggregate JOLs exhibited "forgetting" unless participants were asked to estimate recall rates for several different retention intervals, in which case their estimates mimicked closely actual recall rates. These results and others reported suggest that participants can access their knowledge about forgetting but only when theory-based predictions are made, and then only when the notion of forgetting is accentuated either by manipulating retention interval within individuals or by framing recall predictions in terms of forgetting rather than remembering. The authors interpret their findings in terms of the distinction between experience-based and theory-based JOLs.

[1]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Tradeoff of semantic relatedness and degree of overlearning: differential effects on metamemory and on long-term retention. , 1997, Acta psychologica.

[2]  J. Dunlosky,et al.  Age-related differences in absolute but not relative metamemory accuracy. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[3]  Ian Begg,et al.  Memory predictions are based on ease of processing , 1989 .

[4]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[5]  Douglas J. Hacker,et al.  Metacognition in educational theory and practice. , 1998 .

[6]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Judgments of learning are affected by the kind of encoding in ways that cannot be attributed to the level of recall. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  John Dunlosky,et al.  Empirical analysis of the intrinsic–extrinsic distinction of judgments of learning (JOLs): Effects of relatedness and serial position on JOLs. , 2001 .

[8]  R. Maki,et al.  Metamemory for narrative text , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[9]  John Dunlosky,et al.  Training Programs to Improve Learning in Later Adulthood: Helping Older Adults Educate Themselves , 1998 .

[10]  M. Masson Using confidence intervals for graphically based data interpretation. , 2003, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.

[11]  Larry L. Jacoby,et al.  Unconscious Influences of Memory for a Prior Event , 1987 .

[12]  J. Dunlosky,et al.  Fluency of retrieval at study affects judgments of learning (JOLs): An analytic or nonanalytic basis for JOLs? , 2001, Memory & cognition.

[13]  Lisa K. Son,et al.  Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[14]  M. Carroll,et al.  Influences on metamemory judgements , 1997 .

[15]  T. Perfect The role of self‐rated ability in the accuracy of confidence judgements in eyewitness memory and general knowledge , 2004 .

[16]  Cesare Cornoldi,et al.  Strategies in study time allocation: Why is study time sometimes not effective? , 1993 .

[17]  R. Bjork,et al.  The mismeasure of memory: when retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  I. Begg,et al.  Generating makes words memorable, but so does effective reading , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[19]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed-JOL effect , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[20]  S. Sloman The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. , 1996 .

[21]  R. Bjork,et al.  Illusions of competence in monitoring one's knowledge during study. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  A. Koriat,et al.  Comparing objective and subjective learning curves: judgments of learning exhibit increased underconfidence with practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[23]  F. Strack,et al.  Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Social Behavior , 2004, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[24]  F. Strack The different routes to social judgments: Experiential versus informational strategies , 1992 .

[25]  Raymond S. Nickerson,et al.  How We Know—and Sometimes Misjudge—What Others Know: Imputing One's Own Knowledge to Others , 1999 .

[26]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Anchoring Effects in the Absolute Accuracy of Immediate versus Delayed Judgments of Learning. , 2004 .

[27]  Alice H. Eagly,et al.  Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. , 1989 .

[28]  B. Fischhoff,et al.  Calibration of probabilities: the state of the art to 1980 , 1982 .

[29]  John H. Flavell,et al.  Stage-related properties of cognitive development , 1971 .

[30]  Asher Koriat,et al.  Applied Metacognition: Metacognition research: an interim report , 2002 .

[31]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Dual-process theories in social psychology , 1999 .

[32]  Marcia K. Johnson,et al.  Source monitoring. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[34]  Raymond J. Shaw,et al.  Age differences in predictions and performance on a cued recall task. , 1989, Psychology and aging.

[35]  D. Hultsch,et al.  Age differences in recall and predicting recall of action events and words. , 1996, The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological sciences and social sciences.

[36]  Alan S. Brown,et al.  Information Processing and Cognition: The Loyola Symposium , 1976 .

[37]  L. Jacoby,et al.  Adult Egocentrism: Subjective Experience versus Analytic Bases for Judgment , 1996 .

[38]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Does Living in California Make People Happy? A Focusing Illusion in Judgments of Life Satisfaction , 1998 .

[39]  John Dunlosky,et al.  Does the Sensitivity of Judgments of Learning (JOLs) to the Effects of Various Study Activities Depend on When the JOLs Occur , 1994 .

[40]  I. Erev,et al.  Simultaneous Over- and Underconfidence: The Role of Error in Judgment Processes. , 1994 .

[41]  Derek J. Koehler,et al.  Explanation, imagination, and confidence in judgment. , 1991, Psychological bulletin.

[42]  L. Reder Implicit memory and metacognition , 1996 .

[43]  Nate Kornell,et al.  The dynamics of learning and allocation of study time to a region of proximal learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[44]  Seymour Epstein,et al.  Some basic issues regarding dual-process theories from the perspective of cognitive–experiential self-theory. , 1999 .

[45]  K. Stanovich,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? , 2002 .

[46]  P. Brickman,et al.  Lottery winners and accident victims: is happiness relative? , 1978, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[47]  Robert S. Siegler,et al.  Metrics and Mappings: A Framework for Understanding Real-World Quantitative Estimation. , 1993 .

[48]  J. Baron,et al.  Do Nonpatients Underestimate the Quality of Life Associated with Chronic Health Conditions because of a Focusing Illusion? , 2001, Medical decision making.

[49]  Henry M. Wellman,et al.  Understanding of Mental Processes: A Developmental Study of "Remember" and "Forget" , 1979 .

[50]  J. Flavell,et al.  Young children's understanding of forgetting over time. , 1993, Child development.

[51]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Allocation of self-paced study time and the "labor-in-vain effect". , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[52]  A. Koriat Monitoring one's own knowledge during study : A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning , 1997 .

[53]  S. Sloman Two systems of reasoning. , 2002 .

[54]  L. Brooks,et al.  Nonanalytic Cognition: Memory, Perception, and Concept Learning , 1984 .

[55]  C. L. Olson,et al.  Four-Year-Olds' Understanding of Pretend, Forget, and Know: Evidence for Propositional Operations. , 1976 .

[56]  G Mazzoni,et al.  Do memorability ratings affect study-time allocation? , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[57]  A. Koriat,et al.  Processes underlying metacognitive judgments: Information-based and experience-based monitoring of one's own knowledge. , 1999 .

[58]  R. L. Cohen,et al.  The failure of memory monitoring in a free recall task. , 1991 .

[59]  K. Thiede The Relative Importance of Anticipated Test Format and Anticipated Test Difficulty on Performance , 1996 .

[60]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and cognitive control. , 1975 .

[61]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. , 1984, Psychological bulletin.

[62]  G. Mazzoni,et al.  Applied Metacognition: Autobiographical memories and beliefs: a preliminary metacognitive model , 2002 .

[63]  David M. Sanbonmatsu,et al.  The Subjective Beliefs Underlying Probability Overestimation , 1997 .

[64]  Kerri L. Johnson,et al.  Why People Fail to Recognize Their Own Incompetence , 2003 .

[65]  John Dunlosky,et al.  Encoding fluency is a cue used for judgments about learning. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[66]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[67]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Effect of overlearning on the feeling of knowing is more detectable in within-subject than in between-subject designs. , 1993, The American journal of psychology.