Integrating attribute non-attendance and value learning with risk attitudes and perceptual conditioning

This study integrates multiple decision process strategies (Attribute Non-Attendance and Value Learning) with risk attitudes, perceptual conditioning and overt experience, which, to the best of our knowledge, has not been done before. We propose a way to include multiple heuristics as an alternative to current dominant paradigms, which supports a behavioural view that more than one heuristic simultaneously is at play in attribute and alternative processing. Our results show that including multiple decision process strategies, and integrating of all these components, significantly improves the model’s performance and enhances our understanding of how preferences are made.

[1]  Juan de Dios Ortúzar,et al.  Modeling Discrete Choices in the Presence of Inertia and Serial Correlation , 2007, Transp. Sci..

[2]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Violations of the betweenness axiom and nonlinearity in probability , 1994 .

[3]  D A Hensher,et al.  Experience conditioning in modal choice modelling: does it make a difference? , 2016 .

[4]  W. Greene,et al.  Embedding risk attitude and decision weights in non-linear logit to accommodate time variability in the value of expected travel time savings , 2011 .

[5]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Bounding WTP distributions to reflect the 'actual'consideration set , 2014 .

[6]  A. Tversky,et al.  Weighing Risk and Uncertainty , 1995 .

[7]  Malte Oehlmann,et al.  Stated and inferred attribute non-attendance in a design of designs approach , 2014 .

[8]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Valuing Travel Time Variability within a Rank-Dependent Utility Framework and an Investigation of Unobserved Taste Heterogeneity , 2012 .

[9]  Andrew T. Collins,et al.  Understanding buy-in for risky prospects: incorporating degree of belief into the ex-ante assessment of support for alternative road pricing schemes , 2013 .

[10]  Charles A. Holt,et al.  Risk Aversion and Incentive Effects , 2002 .

[11]  W. Adamowicz,et al.  Are Food Choices Really Habitual? Integrating Habits, Variety‐Seeking, and Compensatory Choice in a Utility‐Maximizing Framework , 2013 .

[12]  A. Tversky,et al.  Preference and belief: Ambiguity and competence in choice under uncertainty , 1991 .

[13]  Arne Risa Hole,et al.  Non-linear mixed logit , 2012 .

[14]  H. Stott Cumulative prospect theory's functional menagerie , 2006 .

[15]  Arne Risa Hole,et al.  A discrete choice model with endogenous attribute attendance , 2011 .

[16]  John M. Rose,et al.  Applied Choice Analysis , 2015 .

[17]  Andrew Daly,et al.  Allowing for heterogeneous decision rules in discrete choice models: an approach and four case studies , 2011 .

[18]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Accounting for attribute non-attendance and common-metric aggregation in a probabilistic decision process mixed multinomial logit model: a warning on potential confounding , 2013 .

[19]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Do familiarity and awareness influence behavioural response: The case of road pricing reform? , 2015 .

[20]  Bo E. Honoré,et al.  Panel Data Discrete Choice Models with Lagged Dependent Variables , 2000 .

[21]  Richard Gonzalez,et al.  Nonlinear Decision Weights in Choice Under Uncertainty , 1999 .

[22]  J. Swait,et al.  The Influence of Task Complexity on Consumer Choice: A Latent Class Model of Decision Strategy Switching , 2001 .

[23]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Experience conditioning in commuter modal choice modelling – Does it make a difference? , 2016 .

[24]  John M. Rose,et al.  Asymmetric preference formation in willingness to pay estimates in discrete choice models , 2008 .

[25]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Revealing Differences in Willingness to Pay due to the Dimensionality of Stated Choice Designs: An Initial Assessment , 2006 .

[26]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Modelling Heterogeneity in Response Behaviour Towards a Sequence of Discrete Choice Questions: A Probabilistic Decision Process Model , 2012 .

[27]  L. Eeckhoudt,et al.  Economic and Financial Decisions under Risk , 2005 .

[28]  D. Hensher,et al.  Revealing the extent of process heterogeneity in choice analysis , 2009 .

[29]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Interrogation of responses to stated choice experiments: Is there sense in what respondents tell us? , 2011 .

[30]  D. Hensher,et al.  Accommodating Risk in the Valuation of Expected Travel Time Savings , 2013 .

[31]  John M. Rose,et al.  Specification issues in a generalised random parameters attribute nonattendance model , 2013 .

[32]  Brett Day,et al.  Ordering anomalies in choice experiments , 2010 .

[33]  A. Tversky,et al.  Advances in prospect theory: Cumulative representation of uncertainty , 1992 .

[34]  J. Pratt RISK AVERSION IN THE SMALL AND IN THE LARGE11This research was supported by the National Science Foundation (grant NSF-G24035). Reproduction in whole or in part is permitted for any purpose of the United States Government. , 1964 .

[35]  David A. Hensher,et al.  Cost thresholds, cut-offs and sensitivities in stated choice analysis: identification and implications , 2011 .

[36]  David A. Hensher,et al.  The implications on willingness to pay of respondents ignoring specific attributes , 2004 .

[37]  H. Timmermans,et al.  Applications of theories and models of choice and decision-making under conditions of uncertainty in travel behavior research , 2014 .

[38]  Stephane Hess,et al.  Treatment of reference alternatives in stated choice surveys for air travel choice behaviour , 2008 .

[39]  John M. Rose,et al.  Identifying commuter preferences for existing modes and a proposed Metro in Sydney, Australia with special reference to crowding , 2011, Public Transp..