A Case of Impaired Naming and Knowledge of Body Parts. Are Limbs a Separate Sub-category?

A left hemisphere stroke patient presented a disproportionate difficulty for body parts knowledge without autotopagnosia. The deficit concerned the lexical-semantic representation of body parts and was most severe for limbs. The ability to gesture was spared and action naming was not more impaired than object naming. On the basis of normal naming latencies, we conclude that limbs are the most vulnerable component of the overall category of body parts. This vulnerability is not explained by unbalanced nuisance variables. More cognitive effort is probably required for the appropriate differentiation of limbs during semantic processing and lexical access.

[1]  M. Farah,et al.  The psychological reality of the body schema: a test with normal participants. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  G. Scotti,et al.  Autotopagnosia: fiction or reality? Report of a case. , 1970, Archives of neurology.

[3]  H. Branch Coslett,et al.  Evidence for Multiple, Distinct Representations of the Human Body , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Erminio Capitani,et al.  Objective versus estimated age of word acquisition: A study of 202 Italian children , 2005, Behavior research methods.

[5]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Selective Sparing of Body Part Knowledge: A Case Study , 1998 .

[6]  M. Dennis Dissociated naming and locating of body parts after left anterior temporal lobe resection: An experimental case study , 1976, Brain and Language.

[7]  H. Gardner The contribution of operativity to naming capacity in aphasic patients. , 1973, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Laurel J. Buxbaum,et al.  Specialised structural descriptions for human body parts: Evidence from autotopagnosia , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[9]  Atsushi Yamadori,et al.  Category-specific comprehension deficit restricted to body parts , 1997 .

[10]  A. Caramazza,et al.  WHAT ARE THE FACTS OF SEMANTIC CATEGORY-SPECIFIC DEFICITS? A CRITICAL REVIEW OF THE CLINICAL EVIDENCE , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[11]  Carlo Semenza,et al.  Impairment in Localization of Body Parts Following Brain Damage , 1988, Cortex.

[12]  Gianfranco Denes,et al.  A category-specific deficit of spatial representation: the case of autotopagnosia , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  Miceli Gabriele,et al.  Batteria per l'analisi dei deficit afasici. B.A.D.A. , 1994 .

[14]  Eleanor M. Saffran,et al.  Knowledge of the human body: A distinct semantic domain , 2002, Neurology.

[15]  H. Goodglass,et al.  Localization of body parts in brain injured subjects , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Carlo Tagliavini,et al.  Lessico di frequenza della lingua Italiana contemporanea , 1972 .

[17]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[18]  Laurel J. Buxbaum,et al.  Representations of the human body in the production and imitation of complex movements , 2004, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[19]  J. Vizmuller-Zocco,et al.  Lessico di frequenza dell'italiano parlato , 1994 .

[20]  E. Renzi,et al.  Imitating gestures. A quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. , 1980, Archives of neurology.

[21]  Laurel J. Buxbaum,et al.  Compensatory coding of body part location in autotopagnosia: Evidence for extrinsic egocentric coding , 2001, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[22]  E. Renzi,et al.  Ideational apraxia: A quantitative study , 1968 .

[23]  A. Sirigu,et al.  Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing. Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia. , 1991, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[24]  M. Laiacona,et al.  Dissociazioni semantiche intercategoriali: descrizione di una batteria standardizzata e dati normativi , 1993 .