Lexical access for concrete and abstract words.

[1]  A. Paivio,et al.  Concrete image and verbal memory codes , 1969 .

[2]  S. Glucksberg Commentary: The functional equivalence of common and multiple codes , 1984 .

[3]  E. Shoben,et al.  Differential Context Effects in the Comprehension of Abstract and Concrete Verbal Materials , 1983 .

[4]  J. Frederiksen,et al.  Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. , 1976 .

[5]  C. A. Becker Semantic context and word frequency effects in visual word recognition. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  D. Balota,et al.  Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  Merrill F. Garrett,et al.  Hemisphere differences in the recognition of closed and open class words , 1983, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  E. Zaidel,et al.  A response to Gazzaniga. Language in the right hemisphere, convergent perspectives. , 1983, The American psychologist.

[9]  B. Underwood,et al.  Perceived frequency of concrete and abstract words , 1973, Memory & cognition.

[10]  M. Gernsbacher Resolving 20 years of inconsistent interactions between lexical familiarity and orthography, concreteness, and polysemy. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[12]  Derek Besner,et al.  Models of Lexical Access in Visual Word Recognition , 1982, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[13]  Paul A. Kolers,et al.  Processing of visible language , 1979 .

[14]  J. T. Linde Picture–word differences in decision latency: A test of common-coding assumptions. , 1982 .

[15]  Judith F. Kroll,et al.  Recognizing words, pictures, and concepts: A comparison of lexical, object, and reality decisions , 1984 .

[16]  S. L. Ehrenreich,et al.  Structure and Search of the Internal Lexicon. , 1979 .

[17]  M. Potter,et al.  Representation of a sentence and its pragmatic implications: Verbal, imagistic, or abstract?* , 1977 .

[18]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  Lexical decision for open- and closed-class words: Failure to replicate differential frequency sensitivity , 1982, Brain and Language.

[19]  A. Paivio,et al.  Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[20]  M S Gazzaniga,et al.  Right hemisphere language following brain bisection. A 20-year perspective. , 1983, American Psychologist.

[21]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Pre- and postlexical loci of contextual effects on word recognition , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[22]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Concepts and Their Surface Representations , 1984 .

[23]  Don L. Scarborough,et al.  Independence of lexical access in bilingual word recognition , 1984 .

[24]  M. Potter,et al.  Time to understand pictures and words , 1975, Nature.

[25]  J. Richardson The effects of stimulus attributes upon latency of word recognition. , 1976, British journal of psychology.

[26]  Walter Kintsch Abstract nouns: Imagery versus lexical complexity , 1972 .

[27]  Juan Segui,et al.  The word frequency effect and lexical access , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  M. Potter,et al.  Lexical and conceptual representation in beginning and proficient bilinguals , 1984 .

[29]  M. Vanderwart,et al.  Priming by pictures in lexical decision , 1984 .

[30]  E K Warrington,et al.  Concrete word dyslexia. , 1981, British journal of psychology.

[31]  J. Levy Language, cognition, and the right hemisphere. A response to Gazzaniga. , 1983, The American psychologist.

[32]  E. Ghatala,et al.  Phenomenal background frequency and the concreteness/imagery effect in verbal discrimination learning , 1976, Memory & cognition.

[33]  John Morton,et al.  Facilitation in Word Recognition: Experiments Causing Change in the Logogen Model , 1979 .

[34]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[35]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[36]  C. Richard Puff,et al.  Handbook of research methods in human memory and cognition , 1983 .

[37]  W. Banks,et al.  Semantic and perceptual processes in symbolic comparisons. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  Don L. Scarborough,et al.  Frequency and Repetition Effects in Lexical Memory. , 1977 .

[39]  W. Lambert,et al.  Language processing in bilinguals , 1978, Brain and Language.

[40]  A. Paivio Imagery and verbal processes , 1972 .

[41]  Edwin J. Kay,et al.  Pronounceability and the visual recognition of nonsense words , 1975 .

[42]  Nancy R. Smith,et al.  Symbolic Functioning in Childhood , 1981 .

[43]  C. T. James The Role of Semantic Information in Lexical Decisions. , 1975 .

[44]  J. Day,et al.  Right-hemisphere language processing in normal right-handers. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  Edward J. Shoben,et al.  CHAPTER 10 – Semantic and Lexical Decisions , 1982 .

[46]  A. Paivio,et al.  Concreteness and imagery in sentence meaning. , 1969 .

[47]  H. Shulman,et al.  Control properties of semantic coding in a lexical decision task , 1977 .

[48]  D. Caplan,et al.  Biological studies of mental processes , 1981 .

[49]  A. Koriat,et al.  Semantic facilitation in lexical decision as a function of prime-target association , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[50]  A. Young,et al.  Age of Reading Acquisition does not Affect Visual Hemifield Asymmetries for Naming Imageable Nouns , 1982, Cortex.

[51]  J. Marshall,et al.  Syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia , 1966 .

[52]  K E Patterson,et al.  Neuropsychological approaches to the study of reading. , 1981, British journal of psychology.