Pitch range declination and reset in turn-taking organisation

This paper examines how pitch range declination and reset contribute to turn-taking organisation. This is part of a broader study of voice prosody, i.e., how pitch, voice quality and temporal features combine for various prosodic functions, both linguistic and paralinguistic. The present study first investigates the effect of the speech unit position in a turn on its pitch range. We also test the effect of the number of speech units in a turn as well as the turn duration on the turn-initial f0 peak height at the beginning of the turn. Our results suggest a pitch range declination trend between the Initial and Median speech units of a turn but a violation of this declination for the Final units of the turn. They also demonstrate that the higher the number of speech units in a turn or the longer the turn, the higher the turn-initial f0 peak height. We discuss our findings along the debate on Projection and Reaction theories and that of Hard vs. Soft pre-planning of speech production. We address how these findings may be useful to formulate a holistic model of prosody and to enhance human-machine interactions.

[1]  Jacques M. B. Terken,et al.  Prosodic realizations of global and local structure and rhetorical relations in read aloud news reports , 2009, Speech Commun..

[2]  Barbara A. Fox,et al.  Practices in the Construction of Turns: The "TCU" Revisited , 1996 .

[3]  Annie Rialland Anticipatory raising in downstep realization : evidence for preplanning in tone production , 2001 .

[4]  Cecilia E. Ford,et al.  Interaction and grammar: Interactional units in conversation: syntactic, intonational, and pragmatic resources for the management of turns , 1996 .

[5]  Ailbhe Ní Chasaide,et al.  The distribution of pitch patterns and communicative types in speech-chunks preceding pauses and gaps , 2014 .

[6]  Julia Hirschberg,et al.  Turn-taking cues in task-oriented dialogue , 2011, Comput. Speech Lang..

[7]  Jan-Peter de Holger N. J. Ruiter,et al.  Projecting the End of a Speaker's Turn: A Cognitive Cornerstone of Conversation , 2006 .

[8]  Maxine Eskénazi,et al.  Optimizing Endpointing Thresholds using Dialogue Features in a Spoken Dialogue System , 2008, SIGDIAL Workshop.

[9]  Mariapaola D'Imperio,et al.  Evidence for soft preplanning in tonal production: initial scaling in Romance , 2006, Speech Prosody 2006.

[10]  Petra Wagner,et al.  Effects of talk-spurt silence boundary thresholds on distribution of gaps and overlaps , 2013, INTERSPEECH.

[11]  A. Cutler,et al.  On The Analysis of Prosodic Turn-Taking Cues , 2018, Intonation in Discourse.

[12]  R. Collier Physiological, correlates of intonation patterns. , 1975, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[13]  J M Terken,et al.  Beyond Sentence Prosody: Paragraph Intonation in Dutch , 1993, Phonetica.

[14]  John Hart,et al.  A Perceptual Study of Intonation , 1990 .

[15]  Wonyong Sung,et al.  A statistical model-based voice activity detection , 1999, IEEE Signal Processing Letters.

[16]  John Local,et al.  Projection and ‘silences’: Notes on phonetic and conversational structure , 1986 .

[17]  Douglas M. Bates,et al.  LINEAR AND NONLINEAR MIXED-EFFECTS MODELS , 1998 .

[18]  Mattias Heldner,et al.  Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations , 2010, J. Phonetics.

[19]  Bruce Connell,et al.  Tone , Utterance Length and F 0 Scaling , 2004 .

[20]  Elizabeth Couper-Kuhlen,et al.  Prosody and sequence organization in English conversation , 2004 .

[21]  E. Schegloff,et al.  A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation , 1974 .

[22]  Johanneke Caspers,et al.  Local speech melody as a limiting factor in the turn-taking system in Dutch , 2003, J. Phonetics.

[23]  M. Selting On the Interplay of Syntax and Prosody in the Constitution of Turn-Constructional Units and Turns in Conversation , 1996 .

[24]  Elizabeth Couper-Kuhlen,et al.  Interactional prosody: High onsets in reason-for-the-call turns , 2001, Language in Society.

[25]  S. Duncan,et al.  Some Signals and Rules for Taking Speaking Turns in Conversations , 1972 .

[26]  V. Yngve On getting a word in edgewise , 1970 .

[27]  Philip Lieberman,et al.  Intonation, Perception and Language , 1968 .

[28]  E. Schegloff Discourse as an interactional achievement : Some uses of "Uh huh" and other things that come between sentences , 1982 .

[29]  Paul Boersma,et al.  Praat: doing phonetics by computer , 2003 .

[30]  Shinji Maeda,et al.  A characterization of American English intonation , 1976 .

[31]  Mattias Heldner,et al.  Pitch similarity in the vicinity of backchannels , 2010, INTERSPEECH.

[32]  Nina Gro nnum Thorsen,et al.  Intonation and text in Standard Danish , 1985 .

[33]  Céline de Looze Analyse et Interprétation de l'Empan Temporel des Variations Prosodiques en Français et en Anglais , 2010 .

[34]  A. Ichikawa,et al.  An Analysis of Turn-Taking and Backchannels Based on Prosodic and Syntactic Features in Japanese Map Task Dialogs , 1998, Language and speech.

[35]  Juan María Garrido,et al.  Prosodic differences in reading style: isolated vs. contextualized sentences , 1993, EUROSPEECH.

[36]  G. Bruce,et al.  Textual Aspects of Prosody in Swedish , 1982, Phonetica.

[37]  A. Kendon Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[38]  John J. Ohala,et al.  Respiratory Activity in Speech , 1990 .

[39]  B. Vaughan,et al.  Naturalistic Emotional Speech Corpora with Large Scale Emotional Dimension Ratings , 2011 .

[40]  Daniel Hirst,et al.  Symbolic coding of higher-level characteristics of fundamental frequency curves , 1995, EUROSPEECH.