Tuning for shape dimensions in macaque inferior temporal cortex

It is widely assumed that distributed bell‐shaped tuning (e.g. Radial Basis functions) characterizes the shape selectivity of macaque inferior temporal (IT) neurons, analogous to the orientation or spatial frequency tuning found in early visual cortex. Demonstrating such tuning properties requires testing the responses of neurons for different values along dimensions of shape. We recorded the responses of single macaque IT neurons to variations of a rectangle and a triangle along simple shape dimensions, such as taper and axis curvature. The neurons showed systematic response modulation along these dimensions, with the greatest response, on average, to the highest values on the dimensions, e.g. to the most curved shapes. Within the range of values tested, the response functions were monotonic rather than bell‐shaped. Multi‐dimensional scaling of the neural responses showed that these simple shape dimensions were coded orthogonally by IT neurons: the degree and direction of responses modulation (i.e. the increase or decrease of responses along a dimension) was independent for the different dimensions. Furthermore, for combinations of curvature‐related and other simple shape dimensions, the joint tuning was separable, that is well predicted by the product of the tuning for each of the dimensions. The independence of dimensional tuning may provide the neural basis for the independence of psychophysical judgements of multidimensional stimuli.

[1]  R. Kirk Experimental Design: Procedures for the Behavioral Sciences , 1970 .

[2]  P. O. Bishop,et al.  Spatial vision. , 1971, Annual review of psychology.

[3]  Dana H. Ballard,et al.  Computer Vision , 1982 .

[4]  Donald D. Hoffman,et al.  Parts of recognition , 1984, Cognition.

[5]  G. Rhodes,et al.  Identification and ratings of caricatures: Implications for mental representations of faces , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[7]  T. Valentine The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology a Unified Account of the Effects of Distinctiveness, Inversion, and Race in Face Recognition , 2022 .

[8]  I. Biederman,et al.  Dynamic binding in a neural network for shape recognition. , 1992, Psychological review.

[9]  David H. Foster,et al.  The cue for contour-curvature discrimination , 1993, Vision Research.

[10]  R. Desimone,et al.  The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.

[13]  J. Ringo Stimulus specific adaptation in inferior temporal and medial temporal cortex of the monkey , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  Gillian Rhodes,et al.  Coding spatial variations in faces and simple shapes: a test of two models , 1998, Vision Research.

[16]  Shimon Edelman,et al.  Representation and recognition in vision , 1999 .

[17]  Martin Arguin,et al.  Conjunction and linear non-separability effects in visual shape encoding , 2000, Vision Research.

[18]  R. Desimone,et al.  Clustering of perirhinal neurons with similar properties following visual experience in adult monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[19]  C. Connor,et al.  Shape representation in area V4: position-specific tuning for boundary conformation. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  A. O'Toole,et al.  Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[21]  R. Vogels,et al.  Inferotemporal neurons represent low-dimensional configurations of parameterized shapes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  C. Connor,et al.  Population coding of shape in area V4 , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Brian J Stankiewicz,et al.  Empirical evidence for independent dimensions in the visual representation of three-dimensional shape. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  T. Poggio,et al.  Neural mechanisms of object recognition , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[25]  P. H. Schiller,et al.  Spatial frequency and orientation tuning dynamics in area V1 , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  M. Tarr,et al.  Visual Object Recognition , 1996, ISTCS.

[27]  M. Behrmann,et al.  Impact of learning on representation of parts and wholes in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[28]  Johan Wagemans,et al.  The effect of category learning on the representation of shape: dimensions can be biased but not differentiated. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  Emmanuel Guigon,et al.  Computing with Populations of Monotonically Tuned Neurons , 2003, Neural Computation.

[30]  G. Rainer,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for detecting and remembering novel events , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[31]  I. Biederman,et al.  Shape Tuning in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  David A. Leopold,et al.  Prototype-referenced encoding of faces in the monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2003 .

[33]  R. Romo,et al.  Correlated Neuronal Discharges that Increase Coding Efficiency during Perceptual Discrimination , 2003, Neuron.

[34]  Irving Biederman,et al.  Parts and relations are analyzable sources of shape variation: Evidence for structural descriptions , 2004 .

[35]  Alexander Grunewald,et al.  The Integration of Multiple Stimulus Features by V1 Neurons , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Tomaso Poggio,et al.  Generalization in vision and motor control , 2004, Nature.

[37]  Charles E Connor,et al.  Underlying principles of visual shape selectivity in posterior inferotemporal cortex , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  Hugh R. Wilson,et al.  Curvature population coding for complex shapes in human vision , 2004, Vision Research.

[39]  M. Harries,et al.  Viewer-centred and object-centred coding of heads in the macaque temporal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[40]  Benjamin B. Kimia,et al.  Shapes, shocks, and deformations I: The components of two-dimensional shape and the reaction-diffusion space , 1995, International Journal of Computer Vision.