Mislocalization of Flashed and Stationary Visual Stimuli after Adaptation of Reactive and Scanning Saccades

When we look around and register the location of visual objects, our oculomotor system continuously prepares targets for saccadic eye movements. The preparation of saccade targets may be directly involved in the perception of object location because modification of saccade amplitude by saccade adaptation leads to a distortion of the visual localization of briefly flashed spatial probes. Here, we investigated effects of adaptation on the localization of continuously visible objects. We compared adaptation-induced mislocalization of probes that were present for 20 ms during the saccade preparation period and of probes that were present for >1 s before saccade initiation. We studied the mislocalization of these probes for two different saccade types, reactive saccades to a suddenly appearing target and scanning saccades in the self-paced viewing of a stationary scene. Adaptation of reactive saccades induced mislocalization of flashed probes. Adaptation of scanning saccades induced in addition also mislocalization of stationary objects. The mislocalization occurred in the absence of visual landmarks and must therefore originate from the change in saccade motor parameters. After adaptation of one type of saccade, the saccade amplitude change and the mislocalization transferred only weakly to the other saccade type. Mislocalization of flashed and stationary probes thus followed the selectivity of saccade adaptation. Since the generation and adaptation of reactive and scanning saccades are known to involve partially different brain mechanisms, our results suggest that visual localization of objects in space is linked to saccade targeting at multiple sites in the brain.

[1]  M. Lappe,et al.  Effects of saccadic adaptation on visual localization before and during saccades , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  H. Deubel Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements , 1995, Vision Research.

[3]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Genetic dissection of Alzheimer's disease and related dementias: amyloid and its relationship to tau , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[4]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Effect of saccadic adaptation on localization of visual targets. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Post-saccadic location judgments reveal remapping of saccade targets to non-foveal locations. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[6]  P. Thier,et al.  Reduced saccadic resilience and impaired saccadic adaptation due to cerebellar disease , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[7]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  Adaptation of voluntary saccades, but not of reactive saccades, transfers to hand pointing movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  C. J. Erkelens,et al.  Selective adaptation of internally triggered saccades made to visual targets , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  B. Gaymard,et al.  Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Mislocalization of Perceived Saccade Target Position Induced by Perisaccadic Visual Stimulation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Motor space structures perceptual space: Evidence from human saccadic adaptation , 2007, Brain Research.

[12]  Masahiko Fujita,et al.  Selective and delay adaptation of human saccades. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[13]  B. Gaymard,et al.  Cortical control of memory-guided saccades in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Eugene McSorley,et al.  The parallel programming of voluntary and reflexive saccades , 2006, Vision Research.

[15]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Visual versus motor vector inversions in the antisaccade task: a behavioral investigation with saccadic adaptation. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  J. Yelnik,et al.  Involvement of the cerebellar thalamus in human saccade adaptation , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[17]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The characteristics and neuronal substrate of saccadic eye movement plasticity , 2004, Progress in Neurobiology.

[18]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Postsaccadic target blanking prevents saccadic suppression of image displacement , 1996, Vision Research.

[19]  Christopher B. Currie,et al.  Visual stability across saccades while viewing complex pictures. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  René M. Müri,et al.  Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades as revealed by lesion studies with neurological patients and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) , 2008, Brain and Cognition.

[21]  A. Fuchs,et al.  The role of the cerebellum in voluntary eye movements. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[22]  K. Shapiro,et al.  The contingent negative variation (CNV) event-related potential (ERP) predicts the attentional blink , 2008 .

[23]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Oculomotor plasticity: Are mechanisms of adaptation for reactive and voluntary saccades separate? , 2007, Brain Research.

[24]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Adaptation of reactive and voluntary saccades: different patterns of adaptation revealed in the antisaccade task , 2009, The Journal of physiology.

[25]  E Kowler,et al.  Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements , 1999, Nature.

[26]  J. Douglas Crawford,et al.  Optimal transsaccadic integration explains distorted spatial perception , 2003, Nature.

[27]  A. Opstal,et al.  Transfer of short-term adaptation in human saccadic eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Harold E. Bedell,et al.  Changes in oculocentric visual direction induced by the recalibration of saccades , 1988, Vision Research.

[29]  S. C. Mclaughlin Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements , 1967 .

[30]  Denis Pélisson,et al.  Behavioral evidence of separate adaptation mechanisms controlling saccade amplitude lengthening and shortening. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  Peter Thier,et al.  Cerebellar-dependent motor learning is based on pruning a Purkinje cell population response , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  S. Grossberg,et al.  A neural model of saccadic eye movement control explains task-specific adaptation , 1999, Vision Research.

[33]  Thérèse Collins,et al.  Eye movement signals influence perception: Evidence from the adaptation of reactive and volitional saccades , 2006, Vision Research.

[34]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Influence of saccadic adaptation on spatial localization: comparison of verbal and pointing reports. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[35]  J. Lynch,et al.  The parieto‐collicular pathway: anatomical location and contribution to saccade generation , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  M. Landy,et al.  The effect of viewpoint on perceived visual roughness. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[37]  M. Banks,et al.  How does saccade adaptation affect visual perception? , 2008, Journal of vision.

[38]  Wilsaan M. Joiner,et al.  Adaptive Control of Saccades via Internal Feedback , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Heiner Deubel,et al.  Transsaccadic memory of position and form. , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[40]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Changes in Control of Saccades during Gain Adaptation , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.