Action and object processing in aphasia: From nouns and verbs to the effect of manipulability
暂无分享,去创建一个
S. F. Cappa | D. Perani | D. Perani | S. Cappa | E. Bates | N. Dronkers | A. Arevalo | A. Butler | E. Bates | A. Arévalo | A. Butler | N. Dronkers | Daniela Perani | Stefano F. Cappa | A. Arévalo | D. Perani | Elizabeth Bates
[1] Elizabeth Bates,et al. Books Recieved , 2022, CL.
[2] A. Caramazza,et al. Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain , 1991, Nature.
[3] C. Sinha,et al. Language, culture, and the embodiment of spatial cognition , 2001 .
[4] D. Perani,et al. Different neural systems for the recognition of animals and man‐made tools , 1995, Neuroreport.
[5] Vittorio Gallese,et al. Listening to Action-related Sentences Activates Fronto-parietal Motor Circuits , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] C. Price. The functional anatomy of word comprehension and production , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] A. Sirigu,et al. The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.
[8] Caterina Pistarini,et al. Verb–Noun Double Dissociation in Aphasic Lexical Impairments: The Role of Word Frequency and Imageability , 2002, Brain and Language.
[9] F. Binkofski,et al. The mirror neuron system and action recognition , 2004, Brain and Language.
[10] Andrew Kertesz,et al. Aphasia and Associated Disorders: Taxonomy, Localization and Recovery , 1979 .
[11] R. Ilmoniemi,et al. Functional links between motor and language systems , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[12] N. A. Borghese,et al. Different Brain Correlates for Watching Real and Virtual Hand Actions , 2001, NeuroImage.
[13] Stefan Geyer,et al. Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] Alex Martin,et al. Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[15] D. Perani,et al. The neural correlates of verb and noun processing. A PET study. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[16] N. Birbaumer,et al. Brain Rhythms of Language: Nouns Versus Verbs , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.
[17] Daniela Perani,et al. Naming deficit for non-living items: Neuropsychological and PET study , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[18] Lorraine K. Tyler,et al. Objects and their actions: evidence for a neurally distributed semantic system , 2003, NeuroImage.
[19] R. Gibbs,et al. Embodied experience and linguistic meaning , 2003, Brain and Language.
[20] Olaf B. Paulson,et al. When Action Turns into Words. Activation of Motor-Based Knowledge during Categorization of Manipulable Objects , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[21] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[22] R. Miall,et al. A system in the human brain for predicting the actions of others , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] G. Rizzolatti,et al. ß Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Mirror , 2003 .
[24] M. Brett,et al. Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.
[26] Andy Clark,et al. Reasons, robots and the extended mind , 2001 .
[27] Cristina Burani,et al. Word reading and picture naming in Italian , 2001, Memory & cognition.
[28] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Motor and cognitive functions of the ventral premotor cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[29] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge , 1996, Nature.
[30] R. J. Seitz,et al. A fronto‐parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI‐study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.
[31] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.
[32] Scott H. Johnson,et al. Imagining the impossible: intact motor representations in hemiplegics , 2000, Neuroreport.
[33] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[34] F. Dick,et al. Voxel-based lesion–symptom mapping , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[35] D. Perani,et al. The Effects of Semantic Category and Knowledge Type on Lexical-Semantic Access: A PET Study , 1998, NeuroImage.
[36] Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al. Encoding words and pictures: A positron emission tomography study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.
[37] Michael Tomasello,et al. Beyond Names for Things: Young Children's Acquisition of Verbs , 1997 .
[38] Lera Boroditsky,et al. The Roles of Body and Mind in Abstract Thought , 2002, Psychological science.
[39] E. Warrington,et al. Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[40] W. Levelt,et al. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation , 1990 .
[41] Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al. Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[42] Stefano F. Cappa,et al. Category-specific Naming Impairment for Artefacts: A New Case , 1998 .
[43] Matthew Flatt,et al. PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .
[44] T. Shallice,et al. Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .
[45] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[46] A. Paivio,et al. Cognitive components of picture naming. , 1996, Psychological bulletin.
[47] Brian MacWhinney,et al. The emergence of language. , 1999 .
[48] Stefano F. Cappa,et al. Word and picture matching: a PET study of semantic category effects , 1999, Neuropsychologia.
[49] Stephen J. DeArmond,et al. Structure of the Human Brain , 1976 .
[50] Anjan Chatterjee,et al. Language and space: some interactions , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[51] G. Gainotti,et al. Evidence for a possible neuroanatomical basis for lexical processing of nouns and verbs , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[52] S. Chen,et al. The dissociation between nouns and verbs in Broca's and Wernicke's aphasia: Findings from chinese , 1998 .
[53] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Activation of human primary motor cortex during action observation: a neuromagnetic study. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[54] Ping Li,et al. The noun-verb problem in Chinese aphasia , 1991, Brain and Language.
[55] R. Berndt,et al. Retrieval of nouns and verbs in agrammatism and anomia , 1990, Brain and Language.
[56] Kara D. Federmeier,et al. Timed Action and Object Naming , 2005, Cortex.
[57] E. Procyk,et al. Brain activity during observation of actions. Influence of action content and subject's strategy. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[58] Alison Gopnik,et al. Names, relational words, and cognitive development in English and Korean speakers: Nouns are not always learned before verbs. , 1995 .
[59] M. Arbib,et al. Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[60] S. Cappa,et al. The breakdown of semantic knowledge: Insights from a statistical model of meaning representation , 2003, Brain and Language.
[61] S. Cappa,et al. Object and action naming in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia , 1998, Neurology.
[62] Helen J. Chenery,et al. Language impairment in Parkinson's disease , 1998 .
[63] D. Perani,et al. The neural correlates of noun and verb processing , 2003, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[64] Karl J. Friston,et al. Anatomic Constraints on Cognitive Theories of Category Specificity , 2002, NeuroImage.
[65] Dedre Gentner,et al. Why Nouns Are Learned before Verbs: Linguistic Relativity Versus Natural Partitioning. Technical Report No. 257. , 1982 .
[66] Anne Cutler,et al. A theory of lexical access in speech production , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[67] Y.R.M. Bastiaanse,et al. How selective are selective word class deficits? Two case studies of action and object naming , 1998 .
[68] A. Glenberg,et al. Symbol Grounding and Meaning: A Comparison of High-Dimensional and Embodied Theories of Meaning , 2000 .
[69] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[70] Dennis L. Molfese,et al. Electrophysiological Correlates of Noun–Verb Processing in Adults , 1996, Brain and Language.
[71] G. Dell,et al. Origins of Paraphasias in Deep Dysphasia: Testing the Consequences of a Decay Impairment to an Interactive Spreading Activation Model of Lexical Retrieval , 1994, Brain and Language.
[72] Ayse Pinar Saygin,et al. In search of Noun-Verb dissociations in aphasia across three processing tasks , 2005 .
[73] G. Vigliocco,et al. A semantic analysis of grammatical class impairments: semantic representations of object nouns, action nouns and action verbs , 2002, Journal of Neurolinguistics.
[74] Michael S Gazzaniga,et al. Within grasp but out of reach: evidence for a double dissociation between imagined hand and arm movements in the left cerebral hemisphere , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[75] Ken McRae,et al. Category - Specific semantic deficits , 2008 .
[76] I. Johnsrude,et al. Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.
[77] O. Hauk,et al. Neurophysiological distinction of action words in the fronto‐central cortex , 2004, Human brain mapping.
[78] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Localization of grasp representations in humans by positron emission tomography , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.