Do action systems resist visual illusions?

Arguments about the relative independence of visual modules in the primate brain are not new. Recently, though, these debates have resurfaced in the form of arguments about the extent to which visuomotor reaching and grasping systems are insensitive to visual illusions that dramatically bias visual perception. The first wave of studies of illusory effects on perception and action have supported the idea of independence of motor systems, but recent findings have been more critical. In this article, I review several of these studies, most of which (but not all) can be reconciled with the two-visual-systems model.

[1]  H. Bülthoff,et al.  Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension , 1994, Current Biology.

[2]  Susan J. Lederman,et al.  The influence of visual illusions on grasp position , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  S P Wise,et al.  The premotor cortex and nonstandard sensorimotor mapping. , 1996, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[4]  J E Cutting,et al.  Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  D P Carey,et al.  Eye–hand coordination: Eye to hand or hand to eye? , 2000, Current Biology.

[6]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[7]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  A Spatially Oriented Decision Does Not Induce Consciousness in a Motor Task , 1998, Consciousness and Cognition.

[8]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  K. Murphy,et al.  The perception of spatial relations in a patient with visual form agnosia. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[10]  Matthew Heath,et al.  The effect of a pictorial illusion on closed-loop and open-loop prehension , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  G. Ettlinger “Object Vision” and “Spatial Vision”: The Neuropsychological Evidence for the Distinction , 1990, Cortex.

[12]  S. Jackson,et al.  The Ponzo illusion affects grip-force but not grip-aperture scaling during prehension movements. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  F. Pavani,et al.  Are perception and action affected differently by the Titchener circles illusion? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  R. Snowden The last grasp? , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  M. Goodale,et al.  Frames of Reference for Perception and Action in the Human Visual System , 1998, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[18]  M. Fahle,et al.  Grasping Visual Illusions: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action , 2000, Psychological science.

[19]  R. B. Post,et al.  Is There Dissociation of Perceptual and Motor Responses to Figural Illusions? , 1996, Perception.

[20]  Leslie G. Ungerleider Two cortical visual systems , 1982 .

[21]  M. Goodale,et al.  Does a monocularly presented size-contrast illusion influence grip aperture? , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  M. A. Goodale,et al.  The role of learned pictorial cues in the programming and control of grasping , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  D. Carey,et al.  More thoughts on perceiving and grasping the Müller–Lyer illusion , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[25]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  Mark Mon-Williams,et al.  The Judd illusion: evidence for two visual streams or two experimental conditions? , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  A. Milner,et al.  Visuomotor sensitivity for shape and orientation in a patient with visual form agnosia , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[28]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Impairment of grasping movements following a bilateral posterior parietal lesion , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Melvyn A Goodale,et al.  Independent effects of pictorial displays on perception and action , 2000, Vision Research.

[30]  J R Tresilian,et al.  Geometrical illusions can affect time-to-contact estimation and mimed prehension. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  R. Weale Analysis of Visual Behaviour , 1983 .

[32]  P van Donkelaar Pointing movements are affected by size-contrast illusions , 1999, Experimental brain research.

[33]  M. Goodale,et al.  Action and Perception in the Visual Periphery , 1997 .

[34]  A. Milner,et al.  Visuospatial processing in a pure case of visual-form agnosia , 1999 .

[35]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979 .

[36]  David A. Westwood,et al.  Pantomimed actions may be controlled by the ventral visual stream , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.