Avoiding spurious findings of nonrandom social structure in association data

Researchers studying animal societies often begin by testing whether a population shows nonrandom social structure, by comparing observed social associations with the predictions of a null model. Association data comprises observations of individuals in groups, which are observed through repeated surveys. Each survey is conducted on a discrete occasion, for example, within 1 day or 1 week. Current null models randomize the interactions among individuals, while preserving two key elements of the data: the number of times that each individual has been seen, and the sizes of observed groups. A critical assumption of existing permutation methods is that each observed group is independent. However, this assumption is often violated. Typically, researchers search a large study area, relative to the distances moved by animals within the sampling occasion. Thus, most individuals will not have had opportunity to change their group associations during a sampling occasion. We show how randomization tests should be modified to account for this nonindependence of group membership. We generated association data sets in which we randomly assigned individuals to groups. We tested these data sets for nonrandom structure using the generally accepted ‘trial swap’ algorithm. We found that spurious conclusions of nonrandom structure occur when we allowed permutation of individuals across sampling occasions, but not when we ‘blocked’ the data by sampling occasion, and constrained our randomizations to permute individuals only among groups within each block.

[1]  Steven J. Schwager,et al.  A comparison of association indices , 1987, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  J. Altmann,et al.  Behavior predicts genes structure in a wild primate group. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  W. Getz,et al.  The socioecology of elephants: analysis of the processes creating multitiered social structures , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  Bryan F. J. Manly,et al.  A Note on the Analysis of Species Co‐Occurrences , 1995 .

[5]  J. Mitani,et al.  Male affiliation, cooperation and kinship in wild chimpanzees , 2000, Animal Behaviour.

[6]  C. Hemelrijk An individual–orientated model of the emergence of despotic and egalitarian societies , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[7]  S. Gueron,et al.  The dynamics of group formation. , 1995, Mathematical biosciences.

[8]  Richard James,et al.  Social networks in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  János Podani,et al.  RANDOMIZATION OF PRESENCE–ABSENCE MATRICES: COMMENTS AND NEW ALGORITHMS , 2004 .

[10]  V. Lehsten,et al.  Null models for species co-occurrence patterns: assessing bias and minimum iteration number for the sequential swap , 2006 .

[11]  James G. Sanderson,et al.  Null matrices and the analysis of species co-occurrences , 1998, Oecologia.

[12]  L. Bejder,et al.  A method for testing association patterns of social animals , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[13]  S. Durant,et al.  Matriarchs As Repositories of Social Knowledge in African Elephants , 2001, Science.

[14]  M. Keeling,et al.  Networks and epidemic models , 2005, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.

[15]  Lars Bejder,et al.  Testing association patterns: issues arising and extensions , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[16]  R. Connor,et al.  Complex social structure, alliance stability and mating access in a bottlenose dolphin ‘super-alliance’ , 2001, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[17]  Ilya R. Fischhoff,et al.  Network metrics reveal differences in social organization between two fission–fusion species, Grevy’s zebra and onager , 2007, Oecologia.

[18]  Hal Whitehead,et al.  Techniques for Analyzing Vertebrate Social Structure Using Identified Individuals: Review and Recommendations , 1999 .

[19]  Wayne M. Getz,et al.  Disentangling association patterns in fission–fusion societies using African buffalo as an example , 2005, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  J. Besag,et al.  Generalized Monte Carlo significance tests , 1989 .

[21]  H. Whitehead,et al.  Testing association patterns of social animals , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[22]  J. Godin,et al.  Association patterns and shoal fidelity in the three–spined stickleback , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Richard E. Strauss,et al.  Cluster analysis and the identification of aggregations , 2001, Animal Behaviour.