Partisan and ideological belief systems: Do they differ?

In this paper a direct comparison is made between the cognitive content of ideological and partisan belief systems. A quasi-experimental design was used in a two-part study. Subjects were randomly assigned to either a partisan or ideological condition and asked to categorize and then scale contemporary leaders, groups, and issues as either Democrat or Republican, liberal or conservative. Results indicate that the meanings of partisan and ideological belief systems are quite similar — their cognitive attributes (issues, groups, and leaders) are interchangeable at the categorical level and highly correlated (r=0.86) in their degree of typicality. Political sophistication is determined to contribute significantly to the degree to which partisan and ideological belief systems are related. For politically sophisticated subjects (Ss) the two belief systems are highly related (r=0.90), whereas for low sophisticates, the belief systems are only moderately related (r=0.50). Sophistication also plays an important role in structuring Ss' own issue preferences. High sophisticates in both the liberal-conservative and Democratic-Republican conditions exhibit a greater level of issue constraint, which can be interpreted as either ideological or partisan constraint.

[1]  M. Lodge,et al.  The meaning of party labels , 1980 .

[2]  P. Converse,et al.  The American voter , 1960 .

[3]  D. Norman,et al.  On the role of active memory processes in perception and cognition , 1975 .

[4]  Mikel L. Wyckoff Belief system constraint and policy voting: A test of the unidimensional consistency model , 1980 .

[5]  R. Abelson Script processing in attitude formation and decision making. , 1976 .

[6]  M. Lodge,et al.  The Difference between Attitudes and Nonattitudes in the Mass Public: Just Measurements?* , 1985 .

[7]  Roger C. Schank,et al.  Scripts, plans, goals and understanding: an inquiry into human knowledge structures , 1978 .

[8]  James A. Stimson Belief Systems: Constraint, Complexity, and the 1972 Election , 1975 .

[9]  Milton Lodge,et al.  Magnitude Scaling: Quantitative Measurement of Opinions , 1981 .

[10]  Geoffrey R. Loftus,et al.  Human Memory , 2019 .

[11]  D. Bobrow,et al.  Representation and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Science , 1975 .

[12]  A. Barton,et al.  Measuring Belief System Structure , 1977 .

[13]  Daniel G. Bobrow,et al.  SOME PRINCIPLES OF MEMORY SCHEMATA , 1975 .

[14]  E. Ebbesen,et al.  Determinants of selective memory about the self. , 1976, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[15]  G. Bower,et al.  Human Associative Memory , 1973 .

[16]  Milton Lodge,et al.  The Breadth, Depth, and Utility of Class, Partisan, and Ideological Schemata * , 1985 .

[17]  W. Kintsch,et al.  The representation of meaning in memory , 1974 .

[18]  Warren E. Miller,et al.  Ideological Interpretations of Presidential Elections , 1979, American Political Science Review.

[19]  E. Tulving,et al.  Organization of memory. , 1973 .

[20]  M. Swafford Three Parametric Techniques for Contingency Table Analysis: A Nontechnical Commentary , 1980 .

[21]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations , 1973 .

[22]  K. Holyoak,et al.  The Structure of Human Memory , 1976 .

[23]  E. Rosch Cognitive Representations of Semantic Categories. , 1975 .

[24]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[25]  Norman H. Nie,et al.  The Changing American Voter. , 1976 .

[26]  Philip E. Converse,et al.  Comment: Rejoinder To Judd And Milburn , 1980 .