How vision and self-motion combine or compete during path reproduction changes with age

Human adults can optimally integrate visual and non-visual self-motion cues when navigating, while children up to 8 years old cannot. Whether older children can is unknown, limiting our understanding of how our internal multisensory representation of space develops. Eighteen adults and fifteen 10- to 11-year-old children were guided along a two-legged path in darkness (self-motion only), in a virtual room (visual + self-motion), or were shown a pre-recorded walk in the virtual room while standing still (visual only). Participants then reproduced the path in darkness. We obtained a measure of the dispersion of the end-points (variable error) and of their distances from the correct end point (constant error). Only children reduced their variable error when recalling the path in the visual + self-motion condition, indicating combination of these cues. Adults showed a constant error for the combined condition intermediate to those for single cues, indicative of cue competition, which may explain the lack of near-optimal integration in this group. This suggests that later in childhood humans can gain from optimally integrating spatial cues even when in the same situation these are kept separate in adulthood.

[1]  P. Zelazo The Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology , 2013 .

[2]  I. Fried,et al.  Direct recordings of grid-like neuronal activity in human spatial navigation , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  D. Knill Robust cue integration: a Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[4]  N. Burgess,et al.  Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  S Levy-Tzedek,et al.  Rhythmic movements are larger and faster but with the same frequency on removal of visual feedback. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  David C Knill,et al.  Sensorimotor Processing and Goal-Directed Movement. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[7]  Nora S Newcombe,et al.  Reorienting When Cues Conflict , 2008, Psychological science.

[8]  Pete R. Jones,et al.  Development of Cue Integration in Human Navigation , 2008, Current Biology.

[9]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Human spatial navigation: cognitive maps, sexual dimorphism, and neural substrates , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[10]  M. Landy,et al.  Measurement and modeling of depth cue combination: in defense of weak fusion , 1995, Vision Research.

[11]  William H Warren,et al.  How You Get There From Here , 2015, Psychological science.

[12]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[13]  Adam N Mamelak,et al.  Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task , 2002, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  Albert R. Powers,et al.  Binding of sights and sounds: Age-related changes in multisensory temporal processing , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Visual influence on path integration in darkness indicates a multimodal representation of large-scale space , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Multisensory integration: psychophysics, neurophysiology, and computation , 2009, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  M. Nardini,et al.  Multisensory uncertainty reduction for hand localization in children and adults. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Differential developmental trajectories for egocentric, environmental and intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory , 2006, Cognition.

[19]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[20]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  THE DEVELOPMENT OF SPATIAL LOCATION CODING: PLACE LEARNING AND DEAD RECKONING IN THE SECOND AND THIRD YEARS , 1998 .

[21]  G. Sandini,et al.  Development of Visuo-Auditory Integration in Space and Time , 2012, Front. Integr. Neurosci..

[22]  David C. Burr,et al.  Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information , 2008, Current Biology.

[23]  G. Sandini,et al.  Poor Haptic Orientation Discrimination in Nonsighted Children May Reflect Disruption of Cross-Sensory Calibration , 2010, Current Biology.

[24]  M. Wallace,et al.  Developmental changes in the multisensory temporal binding window persist into adolescence. , 2012, Developmental science.

[25]  Sheila G. Crewther,et al.  Developmental trends in the facilitation of multisensory objects with distractors , 2015, Front. Psychol..

[26]  Pete R. Jones,et al.  Hearing Where the Eyes See: Children Use an Irrelevant Visual Cue When Localizing Sounds. , 2015, Child development.

[27]  T. Wolbers,et al.  How cognitive aging affects multisensory integration of navigational cues , 2014, Neurobiology of Aging.

[28]  J. Rieser,et al.  Bayesian integration of spatial information. , 2007, Psychological bulletin.

[29]  Gordon E. Legge,et al.  Combining Path Integration and Remembered Landmarks When Navigating without Vision , 2013, PloS one.

[30]  M. Shivdasani,et al.  Susceptibility to the flash-beep illusion is increased in children compared to adults. , 2011, Developmental science.

[31]  M. Nardini,et al.  When vision is not an option: children’s integration of auditory and haptic information is suboptimal , 2014, Developmental science.

[32]  Neil Burgess,et al.  How environment and self‐motion combine in neural representations of space , 2016, The Journal of physiology.

[33]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  A Normalization Model of Multisensory Integration , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  Katrien van Driessen,et al.  A Fast Algorithm for the Minimum Covariance Determinant Estimator , 1999, Technometrics.

[35]  G. DeAngelis,et al.  Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Monica Gori,et al.  Impaired visual size-discrimination in children with movement disorders , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[37]  Russell A. Epstein Parahippocampal and retrosplenial contributions to human spatial navigation , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[38]  D. Angelaki,et al.  A computational perspective on autism , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[39]  Mia Hubert,et al.  LIBRA: a MATLAB library for robust analysis , 2005 .

[40]  Marko Nardini,et al.  Late Development of Cue Integration Is Linked to Sensory Fusion in Cortex , 2015, Current Biology.