Some ruminations on the validation of clinical procedures.

It is becoming almost a cliché to say that “clinical psychology is in a state of ferment,” a remark which is ambiguous as to whether the “ferment” is a healthy or pathological condition. Dr. E. Lowell Kelly finds upon follow-up that about 40 per cent of the young clinicians who were studied in the early days of the Veterans’ Administration training programme now state that they would not go into clinical psychology if they had it to do over again (personal communication). In recent textbooks, such as Garfield’s, one can detect a note of apology or defensiveness which was not apparent even a decade ago (13, pp. vi, 28, 88, 97, 101, 109, 116, 152, 168, 451, and passim). No doubt economic and sociological factors, having little to do with the substance of clinical psychology, contribute in some measure to this state of mind within the profession. But I believe that there are also deeper reasons, involving the perception by many clinicians of the sad state of the science and art which we are trying to practise (17). The main function of the clinical psychologist is psychodiagnosis; and the statistics indicate that, while the proportion of his time spent in this activity has tended to decrease in favour of therapy, it nevertheless continues to occupy the largest part of his working day. Psychodiagnosis was the original basis upon which the profession became accepted as ancillary to psychiatry, and it is still thought of in most quarters as our distinctive contribution to the handling of a patient. One is therefore disturbed to note the alacrity with which many psychologists move out of psychodiagnosis when it becomes feasible for them to do so. I want to suggest that this is only partly because of the even higher valence of competing activities, and that it springs also from an awareness, often vague and warded off, that our diagnostic instruments are not very powerful. In this paper I want to devote myself entirely to this problem, and specifically to problems of validity in the area broadly labeled “personality assessment.” I have chosen the word “ruminations” in my title. It helps from time to time for us to go back to the beginning and to formulate just what we are

[1]  The practical utility of the clinical report. , 1953, Journal of consulting psychology.

[2]  Melba Phillips,et al.  Physics, The Pioneer Science , 1961 .

[3]  Abraham Kaplan,et al.  Definition and Specification of Meaning , 1946 .

[4]  CLASSIC DESCRIPTIONS OF DISEASE , 1933 .

[5]  D. More,et al.  Does TAT add information to interviews? Statistical analysis of the increment. , 1956, Journal of clinical psychology.

[6]  D. Hastings Follow-up results in psychiatric illness. , 1958, The American journal of psychiatry.

[7]  S. Hathaway A study of human behavior: The clinical psychologist. , 1958 .

[8]  W E CHAMBERLAIN,et al.  Descriptive classification of pulmonary shadows; a revelation of unreliability in the roentgenographic diagnosis of tuberculosis. , 1954, American review of tuberculosis.

[9]  A. H. Douthwaite Mistakes in diagnosis. , 1953, Medical world.

[10]  R. Cattell The Description and Measurement of Personality , 1947, Mental Health.

[11]  Donald W. Fiske,et al.  The prediction of performance in clinical psychology , 1951 .

[12]  L. Nash The atomic-molecular theory , 1950 .

[13]  L. Cronbach,et al.  Construct validity in psychological tests. , 1955, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  C. A. Dailey The Effects of Premature Conclusion Upon the Acquisition of Understanding of a Person , 1952 .

[15]  W. W. Rozeboom,et al.  Mediation variables in scientific theory. , 1956, Psychological review.

[16]  P. Hoch,et al.  Pseudoneurotic forms of schizophrenia , 1949, The Psychiatric quarterly.

[17]  A. Kostlán A method for the empirical study of psychodiagnosis. , 1954, Journal of consulting psychology.

[18]  A. Caracciolo,et al.  Reduction-Sentences and Open Concepts , 1955 .

[19]  Paul E. Meehl,et al.  When shall we use our heads instead of the formula , 1957 .

[20]  J. Zubin,et al.  Psychological prognosis of outcome in the mental disorders. , 1954, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[21]  D. Lykken,et al.  A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[22]  M. Lorr,et al.  Factors descriptive of psychiatric outpatients. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[23]  A reply to Patterson. , 1955, Journal of consulting psychology.

[24]  London Id Some consequences for history and psychology of Langmuir's concept of convergence and divergence of phenomena. , 1946 .

[25]  H. O. Schmidt,et al.  The reliability of psychiatric diagnosis; a new look. , 1956, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[26]  P. Ash The reliability of psychiatric diagnoses. , 1949, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[27]  P. Meehl,et al.  Antecedent probability and the efficiency of psychometric signs, patterns, or cutting scores. , 1955, Psychological bulletin.

[28]  Carl G. Hempel,et al.  Fundamentals of Concept Formation in Empirical Science , 1952 .

[29]  Michael Scriven,et al.  Concepts, Theories, and the Mind-Body Problem , 1959 .

[30]  N. Tallent On individualizing the psychologist's clinical evaluation. , 1958, Journal of clinical psychology.

[31]  Michael Scriven,et al.  Definitions, explanations, and theories , 1958 .

[32]  D. Roller The early development of the concepts of temperature and heat : the rise and decline of the caloric theory , 1961 .

[33]  P. Horst,et al.  Pattern analysis and configural scoring. , 1954, Journal of clinical psychology.

[34]  J. Masserman,et al.  Diagnosis and Prognosis in Psychiatry: With a Follow-up Study of the Results of Short-Term General Hospital Therapy of Psychiatric Cases , 1938 .

[35]  B. Mehlman The reliability of psychiatric diagnoses. , 1952, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[36]  J. Loevinger Objective Tests as Instruments of Psychological Theory , 1957 .

[37]  TECHNICAL recommendations for psychological tests and diagnostic techniques. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.

[38]  I. D. London Some consequences for history and psychology of Langmuir's concept of convergence and divergence of phenomena. , 1946, Psychological review.

[39]  L. Eron Frequencies of themes and identifications in the stories of schizophrenic patients and non-hospitalized college students. , 1948, Journal of consulting psychology.

[40]  C. H. Patterson Diagnostic accuracy or diagnostic stereotypy? , 1955, Journal of consulting psychology.

[41]  Paul E. Meehl,et al.  Wanted—a good cook-book. , 1956 .

[42]  W. F. Soskin Bias in postdiction from projective tests. , 1954, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[43]  B. Davenport The semantic validity of TAT interpretations. , 1952, Journal of consulting psychology.

[44]  B. R. Forer,et al.  The fallacy of personal validation; a classroom demonstration of gullibility. , 2011, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[45]  Patterson Ch Diagnostic accuracy or diagnostic stereotypy , 1955 .

[46]  Douthwaite Ah Mistakes in diagnosis. , 1953 .

[47]  S. Garfield,et al.  Introductory Clinical Psychology , 1958 .

[48]  N. Sundberg,et al.  The acceptability of "fake" versus "bona fide" personality test interpretations. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[49]  C. Rogers,et al.  Psychotherapy and personality change. , 1955 .

[50]  O. Diethelm,et al.  Electroencephalographic studies of psychopathic personalities. , 1946, Archives of neurology and psychiatry.

[51]  K. Maccorquodale,et al.  On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. , 1948, Psychological review.