Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied How Does Response Inhibition Influence Decision Making When Gambling?

Recent research suggests that response inhibition training can alter impulsive and compulsive behavior. When stop signals are introduced in a gambling task, people not only become more cautious when executing their choice responses, they also prefer lower bets when gambling. Here, we examined how stopping motor responses influences gambling. Experiment 1 showed that the reduced betting in stop-signal blocks was not caused by changes in information sampling styles or changes in arousal. In Experiments 2a and 2b, people preferred lower bets when they occasionally had to stop their response in a secondary decision-making task but not when they were instructed to respond as accurately as possible. Experiment 3 showed that merely introducing trials on which subjects could not gamble did not influence gambling preferences. Experiment 4 demonstrated that the effect of stopping on gambling generalized to different populations. Further, 2 combined analyses suggested that the effect of stopping on gambling preferences was reliable but small. Finally, Experiment 5 showed that the effect of stopping on gambling generalized to a different task. On the basis of our findings and earlier research, we propose that the presence of stop signals influences gambling by reducing approach behavior and altering the motivational value of the gambling outcome.

[1]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Between-Task Competition and Cognitive Control in Task Switching , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[3]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Impulsive Action but Not Impulsive Choice Determines Problem Gambling Severity , 2012, PloS one.

[4]  M. W. Molen,et al.  A psychophysiological analysis of inhibitory motor control in the stop-signal paradigm , 2001, Biological Psychology.

[5]  F. Verbruggen,et al.  Banishing the Control Homunculi in Studies of Action Control and Behavior Change , 2014, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[6]  J. Duncan Disorganisation of behaviour after frontal lobe damage , 1986 .

[7]  Harold Pashler,et al.  Does working memory load lead to greater impulsivity? Commentary on Hinson, Jameson, and Whitney (2003). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[8]  M. Browne,et al.  Jackpot Expiry: An Experimental Investigation of a New EGM Player-Protection Feature , 2015, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[9]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[10]  Martina Rieger,et al.  Inhibitory after‐effects in the stop signal paradigm , 1999 .

[11]  J. Roache,et al.  Dissociation of physiology and behavior in PTSD. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[12]  A. Jansen,et al.  Training inhibitory control. A recipe for resisting sweet temptations , 2011, Appetite.

[13]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Banishing the Control Homunculus , 2000 .

[14]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[15]  A. Bechara,et al.  A Triadic Neurocognitive Approach to Addiction for Clinical Interventions , 2013, Front. Psychiatry.

[16]  A. Aron,et al.  Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  H. de Wit Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes , 2009, Addiction biology.

[18]  M. Rockloff,et al.  Never Smile at a Crocodile: Betting on Electronic Gaming Machines is Intensified by Reptile-Induced Arousal , 2010, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[19]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[20]  R. Passingham Attention to action. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[21]  B. Balleine,et al.  The Role of Learning in the Operation of Motivational Systems , 2002 .

[22]  M. Bellgrove,et al.  Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience , 2009, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[23]  A. Nobre,et al.  Response Inhibition is Linked to Emotional Devaluation: Behavioural and Electrophysiological Evidence , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[24]  R. Ratcliff Modeling response signal and response time data , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[25]  W. Stroebe,et al.  Stop signals decrease choices for palatable foods through decreased food evaluation , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[26]  Z. Dienes Bayesian Versus Orthodox Statistics: Which Side Are You On? , 2011, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[27]  G. Logan,et al.  On the Use of a Concurrent Memory Load to Measure Attention and Automaticity , 1979 .

[28]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The Common Neural Basis of Exerting Self-Control in Multiple Domains , 2009 .

[29]  T. Robbins,et al.  Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Full of Sound and Fury, Signifying Something: The Impact of Autonomic Arousal on EGM Gambling , 2007, Journal of Gambling Studies.

[31]  M. Vink,et al.  Expectations and violations: Delineating the neural network of proactive inhibitory control , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[32]  Andrea M Philipp,et al.  Control and interference in task switching--a review. , 2010, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  D. Kahneman A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. , 2003, The American psychologist.

[34]  H. Lesieur,et al.  The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SaGS): A New Instrument for the Identification of Pathological Gamblers , 2010 .

[35]  M. Field,et al.  The effects of priming restrained versus disinhibited behaviour on alcohol-seeking in social drinkers. , 2011, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[36]  A. Lawrence,et al.  The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[37]  Chung-Chuan Lo,et al.  Proactive Inhibitory Control and Attractor Dynamics in Countermanding Action: A Spiking Neural Circuit Model , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  L. Pessoa The Cognitive-Emotional Brain: From Interactions to Integration , 2013 .

[39]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[40]  Wim Fias,et al.  Post-error slowing: An orienting account , 2009, Cognition.

[41]  Sally Monaghan,et al.  Review of Pop-Up Messages on Electronic Gaming Machines as a Proposed Responsible Gambling Strategy , 2008, International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction.

[42]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  A computational model of risk, conflict, and individual difference effects in the anterior cingulate cortex , 2008, Brain Research.

[43]  Elizabeth B. Liddle,et al.  Looking before you leap: A theory of motivated control of action , 2009, Cognition.

[44]  Priming a restrained mental set reduces alcohol-seeking independently of mood , 2011, Psychopharmacology.

[45]  P. Whitney,et al.  Impulsive decision making and working memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[46]  R. D. Gordon,et al.  Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.

[47]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[48]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[49]  J. van Limbeek,et al.  The Addiction Severity Index: reliability and validity in a Dutch addict population. , 1989, Journal of substance abuse treatment.

[50]  K. van der Goten,et al.  Interfering with the Central Executive by Means of a Random Interval Repetition Task , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[51]  C. Nederkoorn,et al.  Beer à no-go: learning to stop responding to alcohol cues reduces alcohol intake via reduced affective associations rather than increased response inhibition. , 2012, Addiction.

[52]  H. Aarts,et al.  Using stop signals to inhibit chronic dieters' responses toward palatable foods. , 2011, Behaviour research and therapy.

[53]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Long-term aftereffects of response inhibition: memory retrieval, task goals, and cognitive control. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  G. D. Logan Task Switching , 2022 .

[55]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Decision making under ambiguity but not under risk is related to problem gambling severity , 2012, Psychiatry Research.

[56]  Timothy C Rickard,et al.  Taxing executive processes does not necessarily increase impulsive decision making. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[57]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[58]  G. Logan,et al.  On the ability to inhibit thought and action: general and special theories of an act of control. , 2014, Psychological review.

[59]  Patrick G. Bissett,et al.  Balancing Cognitive Demands: Control Adjustments in the Stop-signal Paradigm Stop-signal Paradigm Post-stop-signal Slowing , 2011 .

[60]  G. Logan,et al.  Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  F. Verbruggen,et al.  Proactive and Reactive Stopping When Distracted: An Attentional Account , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[62]  A. Tversky,et al.  Choices, Values, and Frames , 2000 .

[63]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[64]  Matt Field,et al.  The effects of cue-specific inhibition training on alcohol consumption in heavy social drinkers. , 2013, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[65]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control , 1997 .

[66]  J. Hollerman,et al.  Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[67]  L. Clark,et al.  Risk-avoidant decision making increased by threat of electric shock. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[68]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[69]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  The Phenomenology of Error Processing: The Dorsal ACC Response to Stop-signal Errors Tracks Reports of Negative Affect , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  A. Aron From Reactive to Proactive and Selective Control: Developing a Richer Model for Stopping Inappropriate Responses , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[71]  F. Verbruggen,et al.  Proactive Motor Control Reduces Monetary Risk Taking in Gambling , 2012, Psychological science.

[72]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  On the synchrony of stopping motor responses and delaying heartbeats. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[73]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[74]  Jason B. Mattingley,et al.  Executive “Brake Failure” following Deactivation of Human Frontal Lobe , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[75]  A. Blaszczynski,et al.  A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. , 2002, Addiction.

[76]  Mauricio R. Delgado,et al.  Acute Stress Modulates Risk Taking in Financial Decision Making , 2009, Psychological science.

[77]  Darryl W. Schneider,et al.  Automatic and Controlled Response Inhibition: Associative Learning in the Go/no-go and Stop-signal Paradigms the Go/no-go Paradigm and the Stop-signal Paradigm , 2022 .

[78]  Lorena R. R. Gianotti,et al.  Disruption of Right Prefrontal Cortex by Low-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Induces Risk-Taking Behavior , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[79]  Natalia S. Lawrence,et al.  Stopping to food can reduce intake. Effects of stimulus-specificity and individual differences in dietary restraint☆ , 2015, Appetite.

[80]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to Action: Willed and Automatic Control of Behavior Technical Report No. 8006. , 1980 .

[81]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[82]  Harm Veling,et al.  When approach motivation and behavioral inhibition collide: Behavior regulation through stimulus devaluation , 2018 .

[83]  D. Norman,et al.  Attention to action: Willed and automatic control , 1980 .

[84]  K. A. Hadland,et al.  Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[85]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  The inhibitory control reflex , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[86]  Katrijn Houben,et al.  Overcoming the urge to splurge: influencing eating behavior by manipulating inhibitory control. , 2011, Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry.

[87]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Are the Effects of Response Inhibition on Gambling Long-Lasting? , 2013, PloS one.

[88]  Harm Veling,et al.  Targeting impulsive processes of eating behavior via the internet. Effects on body weight , 2014, Appetite.

[89]  M. Fenske,et al.  Hot or Not: Response Inhibition Reduces the Hedonic Value and Motivational Incentive of Sexual Stimuli , 2012, Front. Psychology.

[90]  E. Perret The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. , 1974, Neuropsychologia.

[91]  A. Dickinson,et al.  Mechanisms of learning and motivation : a memorial volume to Jerzy Konorski , 1981 .

[92]  E. Holmes,et al.  Psychological treatments: A call for mental-health science , 2014, Nature.

[93]  M. Potenza,et al.  Neurobiological considerations in understanding behavioral treatments for pathological gambling. , 2013, Psychology of addictive behaviors : journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors.

[94]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[95]  Sally Monaghan,et al.  Responsible gambling strategies for Internet gambling: The theoretical and empirical base of using pop-up messages to encourage self-awareness , 2009, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[96]  C. L. M. The Psychology of Attention , 1890, Nature.

[97]  Raymond J. Dolan,et al.  Conditioned associations and economic decision biases , 2010, NeuroImage.