Neurobehavioral performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion.

CONTEXT Concern exists about the effect of extended resident work hours; however, no study has evaluated training-related performance impairments against an accepted standard of functional impairment. OBJECTIVES To compare post-call performance during a heavy call rotation (every fourth or fifth night) to performance with a blood alcohol concentration of 0.04 to 0.05 g% (per 100 mL of blood) during a light call rotation, and to evaluate the association between self-assessed and actual performance. DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS A prospective 2-session within-subject study of 34 pediatric residents (18 women and 16 men; mean age, 28.7 years) in an academic medical center conducted between October 2001 and August 2003, who were tested under 4 conditions: light call, light call with alcohol, heavy call, and heavy call with placebo. INTERVENTIONS Residents attended a test session during the final week of a light call rotation (non-post-call) and during the final week of a heavy call rotation (post-call). At each session, they underwent a 60-minute test battery (light and heavy call conditions), ingested either alcohol (light call with alcohol condition) or placebo (heavy call with placebo condition), and repeated the test battery. Performance self-evaluations followed each test. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES Sustained attention, vigilance, and simulated driving performance measures; and self-report sleepiness, performance, and effort measures. RESULTS Participants achieved the target blood alcohol concentration. Compared with light call, heavy call reaction times were 7% slower (242.5 vs 225.9 milliseconds, P<.001); commission errors were 40% higher (38.2% vs 27.2%, P<.001); and lane variability (7.0 vs 5.5 ft, P<.001) and speed variability (4.1 vs 2.4 mph, P<.001) on the driving simulator were 27% and 71% greater, respectively. Speed variability was 29% greater in heavy call with placebo than light call with alcohol (4.2 vs 3.2 mph, P = .01), and reaction time, lapses, omission errors, and off-roads were not different. Correlation between self-assessed and actual performance under heavy call was significant for commission errors (r = -0.45, P = .01), lane variability (r = -0.76, P<.001), and speed variability (r = -0.71, P<.001), but not for reaction time. CONCLUSIONS Post-call performance impairment during a heavy call rotation is comparable with impairment associated with a 0.04 to 0.05 g% blood alcohol concentration during a light call rotation, as measured by sustained attention, vigilance, and simulated driving tasks. Residents' ability to judge this impairment may be limited and task-specific.

[1]  F. Speizer,et al.  Extended work shifts and the risk of motor vehicle crashes among interns. , 2005, The New England journal of medicine.

[2]  J. Rothschild,et al.  Effect of reducing interns' weekly work hours on sleep and attentional failures. , 2004, The New England journal of medicine.

[3]  D. Bates,et al.  Effect of reducing interns' work hours on serious medical errors in intensive care units. , 2004, The New England journal of medicine.

[4]  H. Moskowitz,et al.  Effects of Alcohol and Other Drugs on Driver Performance , 2004, Traffic injury prevention.

[5]  K. Strohl,et al.  The Effects of Sleep Loss and Fatigue on Resident–Physicians: A Multi-Institutional, Mixed-Method Study , 2004, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[6]  J. Shea,et al.  Sleep Behaviors and Attitudes among Internal Medicine Housestaff in a U.S. University-Based Residency Program , 2004, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[7]  D. Baldwin,et al.  Sleep deprivation and fatigue in residency training: results of a national survey of first- and second-year residents. , 2004, Sleep.

[8]  J. Dorrian,et al.  Adaptation of performance during a week of simulated night work , 2004, Ergonomics.

[9]  P. Becker,et al.  Attention and working memory in resident anaesthetists after night duty: group and individual effects , 2004, Occupational and Environmental Medicine.

[10]  J. Gravenstein,et al.  The effect of fatigue on the performance of a simulated anesthetic monitoring task , 2004, Journal of Clinical Monitoring.

[11]  D. Baldwin,et al.  A National Survey of Residents’ Self-Reported Work Hours: Thinking Beyond Specialty , 2003, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[12]  Andy H. Lee,et al.  Assessing the driving performance of older adult drivers: on-road versus simulated driving. , 2003, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[13]  C. Patrick,et al.  Deleterious effects of alcohol intoxication: diminished cognitive control and its behavioral consequences. , 2003, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[14]  P. Succop,et al.  The night float paradigm to decrease sleep deprivation: good solution or a new problem? , 2003, Ergonomics.

[15]  D. Dinges,et al.  The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. , 2003, Sleep.

[16]  R. Killiany,et al.  The effects of consecutive night shifts on neuropsychological performance of interns in the emergency department: a pilot study. , 2003, Annals of emergency medicine.

[17]  Maria L. Thomas,et al.  Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose‐response study , 2003, Journal of sleep research.

[18]  B. Barzansky,et al.  Sleep loss and fatigue in residency training: a reappraisal. , 2002, JAMA.

[19]  W. Riedel,et al.  Differential residual effects of zaleplon and zopiclone on actual driving: a comparison with a low dose of alcohol. , 2002, Sleep.

[20]  T. Grantcharov,et al.  Laparoscopic performance after one night on call in a surgical department: prospective study , 2001, BMJ : British Medical Journal.

[21]  J. Owens,et al.  Sleep loss and fatigue in medical training , 2001, Current opinion in pulmonary medicine.

[22]  A. Maclean,et al.  How do prolonged wakefulness and alcohol compare in the decrements they produce on a simulated driving task? , 2001, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[23]  S. Doran,et al.  Sustained attention performance during sleep deprivation: evidence of state instability. , 2001, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[24]  J Howland,et al.  Effects of low-dose alcohol exposure on simulated merchant ship piloting by maritime cadets. , 2001, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[25]  A M Feyer,et al.  Developing measures of fatigue using an alcohol comparison to validate the effects of fatigue on performance. , 2000, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[26]  A. Williamson,et al.  Moderate sleep deprivation produces impairments in cognitive and motor performance equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication , 2000, Occupational and environmental medicine.

[27]  Munt,et al.  Simulated driving performance following prolonged wakefulness and alcohol consumption: separate and combined contributions to impairment , 2000, Journal of sleep research.

[28]  J. Dorrian,et al.  The ability to self‐monitor performance when fatigued , 2000, Journal of sleep research.

[29]  J Howland,et al.  Effects of low-dose alcohol exposure on simulated merchant ship handling power plant operation by maritime cadets. , 2000, Addiction.

[30]  D. Dawson,et al.  Quantifying the performance impairment associated with fatigue , 1999, Journal of sleep research.

[31]  R. Muelleman,et al.  The occupational risk of motor vehicle collisions for emergency medicine residents. , 1999, Academic emergency medicine : official journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine.

[32]  B. Logan,et al.  Development of dosing guidelines for reaching selected target breath alcohol concentrations. , 1999, Journal of studies on alcohol.

[33]  R B D'Agostino,et al.  Alcohol effects on mood, equilibrium, and simulated driving. , 1999, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[34]  Stephen H. Fairclough,et al.  Impairment of Driving Performance Caused by Sleep Deprivation or Alcohol: A Comparative Study , 1999, Hum. Factors.

[35]  A. Sadeh,et al.  Estimating sleep patterns with activity monitoring in children and adolescents: how many nights are necessary for reliable measures? , 1999, Sleep.

[36]  A. Darzi,et al.  Effect of sleep deprivation on surgeons' dexterity on laparoscopy simulator , 1998, The Lancet.

[37]  D. Baldwin,et al.  Learning, satisfaction, and mistreatment during medical internship: a national survey of working conditions. , 1998, JAMA.

[38]  J. Baranski,et al.  Self‐monitoring cognitive performance during sleep deprivation: effects of modafinil, d‐amphetamine and placebo , 2003, Journal of sleep research.

[39]  D. Dinges,et al.  Rotating shiftwork schedules: can we enhance physician adaptation to night shifts? , 1997, Academic emergency medicine : official journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine.

[40]  D. Dawson,et al.  Fatigue, alcohol and performance impairment , 1997, Nature.

[41]  M. Dodd,et al.  Young doctors' health--I. How do working conditions affect attitudes, health and performance? , 1997, Social science & medicine.

[42]  A. Pack,et al.  Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. , 1997, Sleep.

[43]  J. Pilcher,et al.  Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. , 1996, Sleep.

[44]  D. Baldwin,et al.  Sleep deprivation in senior medical students and first‐year residents , 1996, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[45]  A T McCartt,et al.  The scope and nature of the drowsy driving problem in New York State. , 1995, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[46]  C. Marcus,et al.  Effect of sleep deprivation on driving safety in housestaff. , 1996, Sleep.

[47]  M. Skaredoff,et al.  Residents' performance before and after night call as evaluated by an indicator of creative thought , 1995, The Journal of the American Osteopathic Association.

[48]  M. Schwedler,et al.  Are Postoperative Complications Related to Resident Sleep Deprivation? , 1995, Southern medical journal.

[49]  T. Roth,et al.  Sleepiness and ethanol effects on simulated driving. , 1994, Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research.

[50]  D. Dinges,et al.  Vigilance decrement and sleepiness. , 1994 .

[51]  R. Letz,et al.  Neurobehavioral effects of the on-call experience in housestaff physicians. , 1991, Journal of occupational medicine. : official publication of the Industrial Medical Association.

[52]  J. C. Lynch,et al.  The effects of sleep loss on cognitive performance of resident physicians. , 1990, The Journal of family practice.

[53]  J. Robbins,et al.  Sleep deprivation and cognitive testing in internal medicine house staff. , 1990, The Western journal of medicine.

[54]  W. L. Gill,et al.  Effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive ability and skills of pediatrics residents , 1989, Academic medicine : journal of the Association of American Medical Colleges.

[55]  D. O’Hair,et al.  Sleep deprivation and resident performance. , 1988, JAMA.

[56]  E J Bartle,et al.  The effects of acute sleep deprivation during residency training. , 1988, Surgery.

[57]  R. Reznick,et al.  Effect of sleep deprivation on the performance of surgical residents. , 1987, American journal of surgery.

[58]  David F. Dinges,et al.  Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations , 1985 .

[59]  D J Rohsenow,et al.  The balanced placebo design: methodological considerations. , 1981, Addictive behaviors.

[60]  J. Havard Alcohol and the driver. , 1978, British medical journal.

[61]  W. Dement,et al.  Quantification of sleepiness: a new approach. , 1973, Psychophysiology.

[62]  D. Cahalan,et al.  American drinking practices: A national study of drinking behavior and attitudes. , 1970 .