Basic human values: Inter-value structure in memory

Three experiments examined the latent structure of values. Participants rated the importance of values clustered in pairs. Based on [Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 1–65). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.] circular model, we predicted and found that the time to rate the second value in each pair was shorter when the two values were motivationally congruent or opposing than when the two values were unrelated (Experiment 1). As expected, this was not the case when participants had to compare the importance of values within each pair (Experiment 2). Finally, semantic relatedness between values failed to explain the effects of motivational compatibility (Experiment 3). Taken together, these results reveal a coherent pattern of value relations driven by motivational compatibilities, over and above perceived semantic relatedness.

[1]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  HELP, I NEED SOMEBODY: AUTOMATIC ACTION AND INACTION , 1998 .

[2]  Sonia Roccas Identification and Status Revisited: The Moderating Role of Self-Enhancement and Self-Transcendence Values , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[3]  N. Feather Values in education and society , 1975 .

[4]  V. Gecas,et al.  Value Attributions and Value Transmission between Parents and Children. , 1988 .

[5]  S. Schwartz Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries , 1992 .

[6]  M. Rokeach,et al.  The Nature Of Human Values , 1974 .

[7]  T. Chang Semantic memory: Facts and models. , 1986 .

[8]  M. Robbins,et al.  Attitudes and Values: A Further Test of the Semantic Memory Model. , 1979, The Journal of social psychology.

[9]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Do different metamemory judgments tap the same underlying aspects of memory? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  M. Browne,et al.  Conceptual and Methodological Issues in Testing the Circumplex Structure of Data in Personality and Social Psychology , 1997, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[11]  James M. Olson,et al.  Values as truisms:evidence and implications , 1998 .

[12]  J. Kounios,et al.  Semantic memory and the granularity of semantic relations: Evidence from speed-accuracy decomposition , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[13]  R. Fisher,et al.  Social-Desirability Bias and the Validity of Self-Reported Values , 2000 .

[14]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[15]  B. Verplanken,et al.  Motivated decision making: effects of activation and self-centrality of values on choices and behavior. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[16]  M. Rokeach,et al.  Stability and change in American value priorities, 1968–1981. , 1989 .

[17]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  The teachable language comprehender: a simulation program and theory of language , 1969, CACM.

[18]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[19]  S. Schwartz,et al.  Value priorities and social desirability: Much substance, some style , 1997 .

[20]  M. Rokeach Inducing Change and Stability in Belief Systems and Personality Structures , 1985 .

[21]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Tradeoff of semantic relatedness and degree of overlearning: differential effects on metamemory and on long-term retention. , 1997, Acta psychologica.

[22]  T. O. Nelson,et al.  Do different metamemory judgments tap the same underlying aspects of memory , 1990 .

[23]  James M. Olson,et al.  Relations between Values, Attitudes, and Behavioral Intentions: The Moderating Role of Attitude Function , 1995 .

[24]  Shalom H. Schwartz,et al.  Values and Behavior: Strength and Structure of Relations , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[25]  R. Schvaneveldt,et al.  Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[26]  J. Sinacore Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions , 1993 .

[27]  J. Kounios,et al.  Social value and attitude concepts in semantic memory: Relational structure, concept strength, and the fan effect , 1996 .

[28]  M. Clark,et al.  Research Methods in Personality and Social Psychology , 1990 .

[29]  S. Schwartz,et al.  Value priorities and subjective well‐being: direct relations and congruity effects , 2000 .

[30]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[31]  S. Schwartz Are There Universal Aspects in the Structure and Contents of Human Values , 1994 .

[32]  J. Bargh,et al.  The automated will: nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Does activation really spread , 1981 .

[34]  N. Feather,et al.  Values, Valences, Expectations, and Actions , 1992 .

[35]  J. M. Olson,et al.  Attitudes in social behavior , 2003 .

[36]  D. Meyer On the representation and retrieval of stored semantic information , 1970 .

[37]  M. Yik,et al.  Linking personality and values: The importance of a culturally relevant personality scale , 1996 .

[38]  D. Gilbert,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: Durability Bias in Affective Forecasting , 2002 .

[39]  M. Rokeach The Nature Of Human Values , 1974 .

[40]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. , 1977 .

[41]  U. Schönpflug Intergenerational Transmission of Values , 2001 .

[42]  M. Rohan A Rose by Any Name? The Values Construct , 2000 .

[43]  J. Michela Within-person correlational design and analysis. , 1990 .