Precursor processes of human self-initiated action

ABSTRACT A gradual buildup of electrical potential over motor areas precedes self‐initiated movements. Recently, such “readiness potentials” (RPs) were attributed to stochastic fluctuations in neural activity. We developed a new experimental paradigm that operationalized self‐initiated actions as endogenous ‘skip’ responses while waiting for target stimuli in a perceptual decision task. We compared these to a block of trials where participants could not choose when to skip, but were instead instructed to skip. Frequency and timing of motor action were therefore balanced across blocks, so that conditions differed only in how the timing of skip decisions was generated. We reasoned that across‐trial variability of EEG could carry as much information about the source of skip decisions as the mean RP. EEG variability decreased more markedly prior to self‐initiated compared to externally‐triggered skip actions. This convergence suggests a consistent preparatory process prior to self‐initiated action. A leaky stochastic accumulator model could reproduce this convergence given the additional assumption of a systematic decrease in input noise prior to self‐initiated actions. Our results may provide a novel neurophysiological perspective on the topical debate regarding whether self‐initiated actions arise from a deterministic neurocognitive process, or from neural stochasticity. We suggest that the key precursor of self‐initiated action may manifest as a reduction in neural noise. HighlightsSelf‐initiated action was operationalized in a novel perceptual decision making task.EEG variability decreased prior to self‐initiated action.These findings could be accounted for by a leaky stochastic accumulator model.

[1]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[2]  L. Deecke,et al.  Finger movement versus toe movement-related potentials: Further evidence for supplementary motor area (SMA) participation prior to voluntary action , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  Daniel C. Dennett,et al.  Elbow Room: The Varieties of Free Will Worth Wanting , 1986 .

[4]  Haim Sompolinsky,et al.  Evidence of Change of Intention in Picking Situations , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  Michael X Cohen,et al.  Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice , 2014 .

[6]  Wilfried Lang,et al.  A new method to determine temporal variability in the period of pre-movement electroencephalographic activity. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[7]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  R. Milner Action Structures , 1992 .

[9]  B. Efron,et al.  The Jackknife Estimate of Variance , 1981 .

[10]  N. Daw,et al.  Learning the opportunity cost of time in a patch-foraging task , 2015, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[11]  J A Obeso,et al.  Cortical activity preceding self‐initiated and externally triggered voluntary movement , 1991, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[12]  Patrick Haggard,et al.  What are self-generated actions? , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[13]  Claire Calmels,et al.  Cortical Activity Prior to, and During, Observation and Execution of Sequential Finger Movements , 2006, Brain Topography.

[14]  A. Gorea,et al.  Agency alters perceptual decisions about action-outcomes , 2016, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  J. Farquhar,et al.  Lost in time... The search for intentions and Readiness Potentials , 2015, Consciousness and Cognition.

[16]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Effects of Cortical Microstimulation on Confidence in a Perceptual Decision , 2014, Neuron.

[17]  M. Hallett,et al.  What is the Bereitschaftspotential? , 2006, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[18]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[19]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Cortical activity is more stable when sensory stimuli are consciously perceived , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  P. Haggard Conscious intention and motor cognition , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[21]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Foraging , 2012, Science.

[23]  P. Brown,et al.  Lateralization of event-related beta desynchronization in the EEG during pre-cued reaction time tasks , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  M. Botvinick,et al.  The intrinsic cost of cognitive control. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[25]  P. Haggard Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[26]  C. Sherrington Integrative Action of the Nervous System , 1907 .

[27]  Holger Wiese,et al.  Movement preparation in self-initiated versus externally triggered movements: an event-related fMRI-study , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[28]  Derk Pereboom Free-Will Skepticism and Meaning in Life , 2011 .

[29]  Jose Luis Patino,et al.  Beta-range cortical motor spectral power and corticomuscular coherence as a mechanism for effective corticospinal interaction during steady-state motor output , 2007, NeuroImage.

[30]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related desynchronisation of central beta-rhythms during brisk and slow self-paced finger movements of dominant and nondominant hand. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[31]  Alison Brettle The Evidence of Change , 2014 .

[32]  Reyna L. Gordon Neural and Behavioral Correlates of Song Prosody. , 2010 .

[33]  Zachary F Mainen,et al.  Neural antecedents of self-initiated actions in secondary motor cortex , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[34]  R. Passingham,et al.  Medial frontal cortex: from self-generated action to reflection on one's own performance , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[35]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[36]  M. Hallett,et al.  Prediction of human voluntary movement before it occurs , 2011, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[37]  R. Passingham The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1993 .

[38]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Event-related synchronization of mu rhythm in the EEG over the cortical hand area in man , 1994, Neuroscience Letters.

[39]  R. Passingham,et al.  Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. I. An investigation using measurement of regional cerebral blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  The influence of evidence volatility on choice, reaction time and confidence in a perceptual decision , 2016, eLife.

[41]  H. Heckhausen,et al.  Readiness potentials preceding spontaneous motor acts: voluntary vs. involuntary control. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[42]  P. Nachev,et al.  The neural antecedents to voluntary action: A conceptual analysis , 2014, Cognitive neuroscience.

[43]  G. Kreiman,et al.  Internally Generated Preactivation of Single Neurons in Human Medial Frontal Cortex Predicts Volition , 2011, Neuron.

[44]  F L Mastaglia,et al.  Cortical activity preceding self-initiated and externally triggered voluntary movement. , 1992, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[45]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983 .

[46]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neural Noise and Movement-Related Codes in the Macaque Supplementary Motor Area , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[47]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[48]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[49]  Biyu J. He Spontaneous and Task-Evoked Brain Activity Negatively Interact , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[50]  Daniel C. Dennett Elbow Room: The Varieties of Free Will Worth Wanting , 1984 .

[51]  L. Green,et al.  A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[52]  J. Gold,et al.  The neural basis of decision making. , 2007, Annual review of neuroscience.

[53]  P. Haggard,et al.  How action structures time: About the perceived temporal order of action and predicted outcomes , 2016, Cognition.

[54]  Andrew M. Clark,et al.  Stimulus onset quenches neural variability: a widespread cortical phenomenon , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[56]  L. Deecke,et al.  The Preparation and Execution of Self-Initiated and Externally-Triggered Movement: A Study of Event-Related fMRI , 2002, NeuroImage.

[57]  A. Pouget,et al.  Variance as a Signature of Neural Computations during Decision Making , 2011, Neuron.

[58]  C. Kennard,et al.  Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[59]  Simone Ferrari-Toniolo,et al.  Modulation of Neural Variability in Premotor, Motor, and Posterior Parietal Cortex during Change of Motor Intention , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[60]  L. Nadel,et al.  Conscious Will and Responsibility. A tribute to Benjamin Libet , 2010 .

[61]  M. Husain,et al.  Reward Pays the Cost of Noise Reduction in Motor and Cognitive Control , 2015, Current Biology.

[62]  W. Prinz,et al.  Neural and behavioral correlates of intentional actions , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  Hans Helmut Kornhuber,et al.  An electrical sign of participation of the mesial ‘supplementary’ motor cortex in human voluntary finger movement , 1978, Brain Research.

[64]  M. Hallett,et al.  Asymmetric spatiotemporal patterns of event-related desynchronization preceding voluntary sequential finger movements: a high-resolution EEG study , 2005, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[65]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[66]  A. Leuthold,et al.  Beta-Band Activity during Motor Planning Reflects Response Uncertainty , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[67]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[68]  Byron M. Yu,et al.  Neural Variability in Premotor Cortex Provides a Signature of Motor Preparation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Jeff Miller,et al.  Measurement of ERP latency differences: a comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods. , 2008, Psychophysiology.