Top–Down Enhancement and Suppression of Activity in Category-selective Extrastriate Cortex from an Act of Reflective Attention
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. D’Esposito,et al. Dissecting Contributions of Prefrontal Cortex and Fusiform Face Area to Face Working Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[2] N. Kanwisher,et al. The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[3] M. Chun,et al. Dissociable neural mechanisms supporting visual short-term memory for objects , 2006, Nature.
[4] M. Corbetta,et al. An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study of Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Orienting of Attention , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[5] Marvin M. Chun,et al. When a Thought Equals a Look: Refreshing Enhances Perceptual Memory , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[6] P. Cavanagh,et al. Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] Robert T. Knight,et al. Top-down Enhancement and Suppression of the Magnitude and Speed of Neural Activity , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[8] N. Kanwisher,et al. Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[9] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Neuroimaging a Single Thought: Dorsolateral PFC Activity Associated with Refreshing Just-Activated Information , 2002, NeuroImage.
[10] M. D’Esposito,et al. Directing the mind's eye: prefrontal, inferior and medial temporal mechanisms for visual working memory , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[11] Soojin Park,et al. Refreshing and Integrating Visual Scenes in Scene-selective Cortex , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] A. Nobre,et al. Attentional modulation of object representations in working memory. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[13] R. Marois,et al. Posterior parietal cortex activity predicts individual differences in visual short-term memory capacity , 2005, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[14] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[15] J. Jay Todd,et al. Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex , 2004, Nature.
[16] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Refreshing: A Minimal Executive Function , 2007, Cortex.
[17] J. Duncan,et al. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.
[18] M. D’Esposito,et al. Category-specific modulation of inferior temporal activity during working memory encoding and maintenance. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[19] Denise C. Park,et al. A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli , 2004, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[20] N. Kanwisher,et al. Covert visual attention modulates face-specific activity in the human fusiform gyrus: fMRI study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article An Age-Related Deficit in Prefrontal Cortical Function Associated With Refreshing Information , 2022 .
[22] Yaoda Xu. The Role of the Superior Intraparietal Sulcus in Supporting Visual Short-Term Memory for Multifeature Objects , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[23] Adam Gazzaley,et al. Functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. , 2007, Cerebral cortex.
[24] Russell A. Epstein,et al. Differential parahippocampal and retrosplenial involvement in three types of visual scene recognition. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[25] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Second Thoughts versus Second Looks: An Age-Related Deficit in Reflectively Refreshing Just-Activated Information , 2002, Psychological science.
[26] Jeffrey W. Cooney,et al. Top-down suppression deficit underlies working memory impairment in normal aging , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.
[27] T. Allison,et al. Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[28] R. Berndt,et al. Working memory retention systems: a state of activated long-term memory. , 2003, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[29] J. Jay Todd,et al. Posterior parietal cortex activity predicts individual differences in visual short-term memory capacity , 2010 .
[30] C. Curtis,et al. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[31] Karl J. Friston,et al. The choice of basis functions in event-related fMRI , 2001, NeuroImage.
[32] Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al. Attention systems and the organization of the human parietal cortex , 2001, NeuroImage.
[33] Russell A. Epstein,et al. Where Am I Now? Distinct Roles for Parahippocampal and Retrosplenial Cortices in Place Recognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] B. Postle,et al. Seeking the Neural Substrates of Visual Working Memory Storage , 2003, Cortex.
[35] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. Age differences in brain activity during perceptual versus reflective attention , 2010, Neuroreport.
[36] H. Intraub,et al. Beyond the Edges of a View: Boundary Extension in Human Scene-Selective Visual Cortex , 2007, Neuron.
[37] Marcia K. Johnson,et al. A brief thought can modulate activity in extrastriate visual areas: Top-down effects of refreshing just-seen visual stimuli , 2007, NeuroImage.
[38] E. Miller,et al. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.
[39] Nancy Kanwisher,et al. A cortical representation of the local visual environment , 1998, Nature.