Multisensory numerosity judgments for visual and tactile stimuli
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. V. Erp,et al. Vibrotactile in-vehicle navigation system , 2004 .
[2] Hong Z. Tan,et al. The Body Surface as a Communication System: The State of the Art after 50 Years , 2007, PRESENCE: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments.
[3] C T WHITE,et al. Temporal numerosity: IV. A comparison of the major senses. , 1959, Journal of Experimental Psychology.
[4] Y. Rossetti,et al. Ameliorating neglect with prism adaptation: visuo-manual and visuo-verbal measures , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[5] Michael J. Wright,et al. Limitations for Change Detection in Multiple Gabor Targets , 2000 .
[6] E C Lechelt,et al. Pulse Number Discrimination in Tactile Spatio-Temporal Patterns , 1974, Perceptual and motor skills.
[7] C. Spence,et al. Modality-specific auditory and visual temporal processing deficits , 2002, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[8] John Duncan,et al. Within-modality and cross-modality attentional blinks in a simple discrimination task , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[9] J. Armington,et al. Temporal numerosity. II. Evidence for central factors influencing perceived number. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[10] Giuseppe di Pellegrino,et al. Neuropsychological Evidence of an Integrated Visuotactile Representation of Peripersonal Space in Humans , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Jon Driver,et al. Attentional competition between modalities: extinction between touch and vision after right hemisphere damage , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[12] E. M. Jensen,et al. The subitizing and counting of visually presented fields of dots. , 1950 .
[13] P Viviani,et al. Choice reaction times for temporal numerosity. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] C. Spence,et al. Seeing Your Own Touched Hands in a Mirror Modulates Cross-Modal Interactions , 2002, Psychological science.
[15] J R Hennessy. Cutaneous Sensitivity Communications1 , 1966, Human factors.
[16] F. Gregory Ashby,et al. Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition? , 1992, Psychological research.
[17] Daniel Gopher,et al. On the Economy of the Human Processing System: A Model of Multiple Capacity. , 1977 .
[18] H. Ono,et al. Depth perception as a function of motion parallax and absolute-distance information. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] T. Hirahara,et al. One, two, many—Judging the number of concurrent talkers. , 1996 .
[20] J. G. Hollands,et al. Engineering Psychology and Human Performance , 1984 .
[21] S Lakatos,et al. Time—distance relations in shifting attention between locations on one’s body , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[22] B. B. Morgan,et al. Masking of Cutaneous Sensations in Multiple Stimulus Presentations , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.
[23] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. What enumeration studies can show us about spatial attention: evidence for limited capacity preattentive processing. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[24] E. Miller,et al. Analog Numerical Representations in Rhesus Monkeys: Evidence for Parallel Processing , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[25] F. A. Geldard,et al. MULTIPLE CUTANEOUS STIMULATION: THE DISCRIMINATION OF VIBRATORY PATTERNS. , 1965, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.
[26] P. Haggard,et al. Facilitated Processing of Visual Stimuli Associated with the Body , 2004, Perception.
[27] Perceived number and evoked cortical potentials. , 1968 .
[28] G Vallar,et al. Anatomical correlates of visual and tactile extinction in humans: a clinical CT scan study. , 1994, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.
[29] C. Spence,et al. Attention and the crossmodal construction of space , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[30] C. Spence,et al. Exogenous spatial cuing studies of human crossmodal attention and multisensory integration , 2004 .
[31] Brian Butterworth,et al. Are Subitizing and Counting Implemented as Separate or Functionally Overlapping Processes? , 2002, NeuroImage.
[32] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Afferent properties of periarcuate neurons in macaque monkeys. I. Somatosensory responses , 1981, Behavioural Brain Research.
[33] Robert Edward Taubman,et al. Studies in Judged Number: II. The Judgment of Visual Number , 1950 .
[34] G. Mandler,et al. Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[35] R. Desimone,et al. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[36] C. Spence,et al. Visual capture of apparent limb position influences tactile temporal order judgments , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.
[37] F. Colavita. Human sensory dominance , 1974 .
[38] Garner Wr. The accuracy of counting repeated short tones. , 1951 .
[39] Hong Z. Tan,et al. Tactile change detection , 2005, First Joint Eurohaptics Conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems. World Haptics Conference.
[40] Ryan M. Traylor,et al. A Haptic Back Display for Attentional and Directional Cueing , 2003 .
[41] Kazuo Hiraki,et al. Auditory-visual intermodal matching of small numerosities in 6-month-old infants. , 2005, Developmental science.
[42] Hendrik A. H. C. van Veen,et al. Tactile Information Presentation in the Cockpit , 2000, Haptic Human-Computer Interaction.
[43] E. Lechelt. Spatial numerosity discrimination as contingent upon sensory and extrinsic factors , 1971 .
[44] E. Spelke,et al. Numerical abstraction by human infants , 1990, Cognition.
[45] Edwin A. Fleishman,et al. Psychology and human performance. , 1959 .
[46] C T WHITE,et al. Temporal numerosity. III. Auditory perception of number. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.
[47] F. Campbell,et al. The Dependence of the Visual Numerosity Limit on Orientation, Colour, and Grouping in the Stimulus , 1976, Perception.
[48] W R GARNER. The accuracy of counting repeated short tones. , 1951, Journal of experimental psychology.
[49] E. Lechelt. Temporal numerosity discrimination: intermodal comparisons revisited. , 1975, British journal of psychology.
[50] R Rothkegel,et al. Subitizing and its subprocesses , 2000, Psychological research.
[51] C. Spence,et al. Numerosity Judgments for Tactile Stimuli Distributed over the Body Surface , 2006, Perception.
[52] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.
[53] Jon Driver,et al. Crossmodal Spatial Attention: Evidence from Human Performance , 2004 .
[54] Georg v. Békésy,et al. Neural Funneling along the Skin and between the Inner and Outer Hair Cells of the Cochlea , 1959 .
[55] Jon Driver,et al. Multiple resources and multimodal interface design , 1999 .
[56] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Auditory localization aftereffects. , 1996 .
[57] Z W Pylyshyn,et al. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.
[58] C. Spence,et al. Spatial constraints on visual-tactile cross-modal distractor congruency effects , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[59] J W Hill,et al. Processing of tactual and visual point stimuli sequentially presented at high rates. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.
[60] J. Fodor. The Modularity of mind. An essay on faculty psychology , 1986 .
[61] Hong Z. Tan,et al. When visual transients impair tactile change detection: A novel case of crossmodal change blindness? , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.
[62] E. L. Kaufman,et al. The discrimination of visual number. , 1949, The American journal of psychology.
[63] D J Newman,et al. A tactile display for international space station (ISS) extravehicular activity (EVA). , 2000, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[64] Gavriel Salvendy,et al. Handbook of human factors. , 1987 .
[65] Felice L. Bedford,et al. Analysis of a constraint on perception, cognition, and development: one object, one place, one time. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[66] Hong Z. Tan,et al. Using spatial vibrotactile cues to direct visual attention in driving scenes , 2005 .
[67] N. Cowan. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[68] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. The Structure of Attentional Resources , 1980 .
[69] Maryanne Martin,et al. Attention to words in different modalities: four-channel presentation with physical and semantic selection. , 1980, Acta psychologica.
[70] Elisabetta Làdavas,et al. Seeing where your hands are , 1997, Nature.
[71] E. Macaluso,et al. Spatial attention and crossmodal interactions between vision and touch , 2001, Neuropsychologia.
[72] C T White,et al. Temporal numerosity and the psychological unit of duration. , 1963, Psychological monographs.
[73] R. Rafal,et al. A systematic study of visual extinction. Between- and within-field deficits of attention in hemispatial neglect. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[74] Robert Edward Taubman,et al. Studies in Judged Number: I. The Judgment of Auditory Number , 1950 .
[75] J. Cantlon,et al. Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.
[76] Andreas Nieder,et al. The Number Domain— Can We Count on Parietal Cortex? , 2004, Neuron.
[77] Joos Vos,et al. Effect on numerosity judgment of grouping of tones by auditory channels , 1979, Perception & psychophysics.
[78] Tony J. Simon,et al. Computational evidence for the subitizing phenomenon as an emergent property of the human cognitive architecture , 2000, Cogn. Sci..
[79] C. Spence,et al. Multisensory prior entry. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[80] Elizabeth M Brannon,et al. The multisensory representation of number in infancy. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[81] C. Spence,et al. Visual dominance and attention: The Colavita effect revisited , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[82] P. Bertelson,et al. The psychology of multimodal perception , 2004 .
[83] F. Campbell,et al. The Magic Number 4 ± 0: A New Look at Visual Numerosity Judgements , 1976, Perception.
[84] C. Packer,et al. Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.
[85] C. Spence,et al. The cost of expecting events in the wrong sensory modality , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[86] C. Spence,et al. The failure to detect tactile change: A tactile analogue of visual change blindness , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[87] C. Spence,et al. Crossmodal Space and Crossmodal Attention , 2004 .
[88] C T WHITE,et al. Temporal numerosity: I. Perceived number as a function of flash number and rate. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.
[89] M. Posner,et al. Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[90] Christopher D. Wickens,et al. Multiple resources and performance prediction , 2002 .
[91] Ronald A. Rensink. Change detection. , 2002, Annual review of psychology.
[92] T. B. Posey,et al. Numerosity Discrimination of Tactile Stimuli , 1976, Perceptual and motor skills.
[93] Michael S Vitevitch,et al. Change deafness: the inability to detect changes between two voices. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[94] C. Spence,et al. Attentional limitations in processing sequentially presented vibrotactile targets , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[95] W. STANLEY JEVONS,et al. The Power of Numerical Discrimination , 1871, Nature.
[96] S. Dehaene,et al. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[97] Anders M. Dale,et al. Optical Imaging of a Tactile Illusion in Area 3b of the Primary Somatosensory Cortex , 2003 .
[98] E. Macaluso,et al. Functional imaging of crossmodal spatial representations and crossmodal spatial attention , 2004 .
[99] C D Wickens,et al. Codes and Modalities in Multiple Resources: A Success and a Qualification , 1988, Human factors.
[100] A. Kleinschmidt,et al. A Supramodal Number Representation in Human Intraparietal Cortex , 2003, Neuron.
[101] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[102] S L Wolf,et al. Doing It with Mirrors: A Case Study of a Novel Approach to Neurorehabilitation , 2000, Neurorehabilitation and neural repair.
[103] G. A. Miller. THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .
[104] Andrew Simpson,et al. Subitizing in Tactile Perception , 2006, Psychological science.
[105] Donald L. Fisher,et al. Central capacity limits in consistent mapping, visual search tasks: Four channels or more? , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.
[106] J. D. Balakrishnan,et al. Is subitizing a unique numerical ability? , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[107] J.B.F. van Erp,et al. A multi-purpose tactile vest for astronauts in the international space station , 2003 .
[108] E. P. Gardner,et al. Sensory funneling. II. Cortical neuronal representation of patterned cutaneous stimuli. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.
[109] Andreas Nieder,et al. A parieto-frontal network for visual numerical information in the monkey. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[110] Influence of an Irrelevant Stimulus Attribute on Numerosity Judgments , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.
[111] C. Koch,et al. Competition and selection during visual processing of natural scenes and objects. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[112] Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al. Monkeys Match the Number of Voices They Hear to the Number of Faces They See , 2005, Current Biology.