Images of time: temporal aspects of auditory and movement imagination

Research on mental imagery has shown that when we imagine something, the related neural processes overlap with those related to actually perceiving or performing that same percept or action (Kosslyn et al., 2001). Although visual imagery has long been the dominant modality for the investigation of sensory imagery, involvement of modality-specific brain regions (i.e., visual areas being implicated in visual imagery, and so on) has now also been reported for auditory, olfactory and tactile imagery (Halpern, 2001; Plailly et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2014). Movement or action imagery has informed theories of action representation (Jeannerod and Decety, 1995) and has recently gained interest in the context of mental practice for expert skill acquisition such as sports or surgery (Cocks et al., 2014) and movement rehabilitation (Malouin and Richards, 2010). Increasingly, the neural underpinnings of imagery have become clearer, and both modality-specific and modality-unspecific neural activations related to imagery have been found (Daselaar et al., 2010; Zvyagintsev et al., 2013). However, the growing body of neuroimaging literature on imagination has yet to include an account of temporal imagery. Although the reproduction of time intervals has been a research topic of interest, the contribution of temporal imagery—namely internal timekeeping or creating the temporal aspects of imagery in other modalities—is largely unexplored. As such it is unclear whether, similar to other perceptual modalities, imagery for time shares cerebral substrate with brain networks involved in time perception or regularity detection. Specifically for actions or sounds, the temporal structure of the imagined stimulus or action is crucial in conjuring a faithful image, suggesting that in scientific findings of auditory and motor imagery, temporal imagery is included. The temporal patterns we create internally arguably lie at the basis of any self-paced movement, be it a fast sprint, a musical performance, or an easy walk in the park. Here, I argue that the shared components between movement and auditory (specifically music) imagery may offer a window into timing and temporal skills, which may carry cognitive importance beyond movement or auditory functions. Whereas the reciprocal influences of rhythmic sound and movement have been shown, with rhythmic movement supporting auditory acuity (Su and Poppel, 2012) and rhythmic sound supporting movement efficiency (Bood et al., 2013), few studies have looked at the extent to which imagery can functionally replace perception or action and show similar interactions, which may point to shared processing in their temporal structure. Music imagination was reported to affect finger tapping similarly to perceived music (Repp, 2001) and has been used to support movement in clinical settings (Satoh and Kuzuhara, 2008). Anecdotal observation from music imagery experiments (such as described in Schaefer et al., 2011b), suggests that tapping a finger along to music imagery supported accurate imagery timing, and auditory cues are reported to increase motor imagery vividness (Heremans et al., 2009). Furthermore, motor timing skills and anticipatory music imagery ability appear to be related (Pecenka and Keller, 2009; Keller and Appel, 2010). These findings suggest that the links between motor and music processing may extend into imagined stimuli or actions, and the contribution of the current piece is to bring together findings from temporal processing, music and motor research, and to assert that firstly, the commonalities between music and motor imagery may reveal more about internally driven timing mechanisms, and secondly, one might speculate that these internal timing patterns are related to expertise, and as such may be seen in the context of findings on temporal skills and cognitive abilities (Madison et al., 2009; Holm et al., 2011).

[1]  David J. Hargreaves,et al.  Musical imagination: Perception and production, beauty and creativity , 2012 .

[2]  P. Jackson,et al.  The neural network of motor imagery: An ALE meta-analysis , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[3]  Dirk Ostwald,et al.  Imaging tactile imagery: Changes in brain connectivity support perceptual grounding of mental images in primary sensory cortices , 2014, NeuroImage.

[4]  J. Larue,et al.  Relation Between Sport and Spatial Imagery: Comparison of Three Groups of Participants , 2004, The Journal of psychology.

[5]  A. Guillot,et al.  How does motor imagery speed affect motor performance times? Evaluating the effects of task specificity , 2011 .

[6]  R. Schaefer,et al.  Music perception and imagery in EEG: alpha band effects of task and stimulus. , 2011, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[7]  B. Ross,et al.  Endogenous Neuromagnetic Activity for Mental Hierarchy of Timing , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  R. Zatorre,et al.  When that tune runs through your head: a PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  Peter Feys,et al.  Facilitation of motor imagery through movement-related cueing , 2009, Brain Research.

[10]  John Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of prospective time interval estimation: the role of cognition, attention, and learning. , 2007, Psychological review.

[11]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mental motor imagery: a window into the representational stages of action , 1995, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[12]  Cyriel M. A. Pennartz,et al.  Modality-specific and modality-independent components of the human imagery system , 2010, NeuroImage.

[13]  C. Richards,et al.  Mental Practice for Relearning Locomotor Skills , 2010, Physical Therapy.

[14]  Edwin Gordon,et al.  The Musical Aptitude Profile , 1967 .

[15]  Martin Wiener,et al.  The image of time: A voxel-wise meta-analysis , 2010, NeuroImage.

[16]  Aymeric Guillot,et al.  Understanding the timing of motor imagery: recent findings and future directions , 2012 .

[17]  C. Moulton,et al.  What surgeons can learn from athletes: mental practice in sports and surgery. , 2014, Journal of surgical education.

[18]  Peter E Keller,et al.  Auditory Pitch Imagery and Its Relationship to Musical Synchronization , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[19]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  The cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery , 1995, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Timothy D. Griffiths,et al.  Distinct Neural Substrates of Duration-Based and Beat-Based Auditory Timing , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  A. Friederici,et al.  Time Perception and Motor Timing: A Common Cortical and Subcortical Basis Revealed by fMRI , 2000, NeuroImage.

[22]  C. Wakefield,et al.  A timely review of a key aspect of motor imagery: a commentary on Guillot et al. (2012) , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[23]  G. Madison,et al.  Intelligence and temporal accuracy of behaviour: unique and shared associations with reaction time and motor timing , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  R. Schaefer,et al.  Decomposing rhythm processing: electroencephalography of perceived and self-imposed rhythmic patterns , 2010, Psychological research.

[25]  C. Delon-Martin,et al.  Experience induces functional reorganization in brain regions involved in odor imagery in perfumers , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Effects of music perception and imagery on sensorimotor synchronization with complex timing patterns. , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[27]  J. Rauschecker,et al.  Brain Activation during Anticipation of Sound Sequences , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Newman Timely review , 1976, Nature.

[29]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Bucknell Digital Commons Bucknell Digital Commons Mental reversal of imagined melodies: A role for the posterior Mental reversal of imagined melodies: A role for the posterior parietal cortex parietal cortex , 2022 .

[30]  E. Pöppel,et al.  Body movement enhances the extraction of temporal structures in auditory sequences , 2011, Psychological Research.

[31]  J. van der Kamp,et al.  The Power of Auditory-Motor Synchronization in Sports: Enhancing Running Performance by Coupling Cadence with the Right Beats , 2013, PloS one.

[32]  P. Keller,et al.  Individual Differences, Auditory Imagery, and the Coordination of Body Movements and Sounds in Musical Ensembles , 2010 .

[33]  E. Schellenberg,et al.  Short-Term Music Training Enhances Verbal Intelligence and Executive Function , 2011, Psychological science.

[34]  R. Ivry The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[35]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural foundations of imagery , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  Sukhvinder S. Obhi,et al.  The Importance of Temporal Structure and Rhythm for the Optimum Performance of Motor Skills: A New Focus for Practitioners of Sport Psychology , 2009 .

[37]  A. Halpern,et al.  Cerebral Substrates of Musical Imagery , 2001, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[38]  Peter Essens,et al.  Perception of Temporal Patterns , 1985 .

[39]  S. Kuzuhara,et al.  Training in Mental Singing while Walking Improves Gait Disturbance in Parkinson’s Disease Patients , 2008, European Neurology.

[40]  Robert J. Zatorre,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Perception, Imagery, and Memory for Familiar Tunes , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  J. Devin McAuley,et al.  Neural bases of individual differences in beat perception , 2009, NeuroImage.

[42]  Guy Madison,et al.  Correlations between intelligence and components of serial timing variability , 2008 .

[43]  Alexander T Sack,et al.  Brain networks underlying mental imagery of auditory and visual information , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[44]  P. Cook,et al.  Memory for musical tempo: Additional evidence that auditory memory is absolute , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.

[45]  Petr Janata,et al.  Acuity of auditory images in pitch and time , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  Peter E Keller,et al.  Mental imagery in music performance: underlying mechanisms and potential benefits , 2012, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.