Two Sides of the Coin: Examining the Role of Character-Similarity and Identification in the Effects of a Narrative on Children’s Learning and Bias

When identifying with a character in a narrative, one experiences the narrative from the character’s perspective, as if the events were happening to oneself. Many researchers and theorists have suggested that a character’s similarity to the audience member should be related to identification. Although some research supports this relationship, it has rarely been tested directly. Additionally, higher identification may lead to more learning from a narrative. These two proposed links, between character similarity and identification, and between identification and learning, suggest that children may learn more from narratives that contain characters who are similar to them than from narratives with dissimilar characters. There is, however, another side to the coin: Reading about (and perhaps identifying with) dissimilar characters may decrease children’s outgroup biases. This study tests these two possibilities, examining race as a point of similarity. Findings suggest that (1) White children may be more likely to identify with own-race characters than with other-race characters, (2) White children do not appear to learn more from stories with own-race characters than those with other-race characters when knowledge is probed via specific questions, but do freely recall more information from stories with own-race characters, and (3) Exposure to a storybook with a Black main character does not reduce White children’s racial bias. These findings inform our understanding of the nature and effects of children’s identification with ownand other-race characters.

[1]  Benjamin K. Johnson,et al.  Temporarily Expanding the Boundaries of the Self: Motivations for Entering the Story World and Implications for Narrative Effects , 2014 .

[2]  Rebecca A. Dore,et al.  Children's racial bias in perceptions of others' pain. , 2014, The British journal of developmental psychology.

[3]  Carolyn A. Denton,et al.  The construction of visual-spatial situation models in children's reading and their relation to reading comprehension. , 2014, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[4]  Riva Tukachinsky,et al.  Experimental Manipulation of Psychological Involvement with Media , 2014 .

[5]  D. Johnson,et al.  Reading Narrative Fiction Reduces Arab-Muslim Prejudice and Offers a Safe Haven From Intergroup Anxiety , 2013 .

[6]  Fenja V. Ziegler,et al.  Stepping into someone else's shoes: Children create spatial mental models from the protagonist's point of view , 2013 .

[7]  Hans Hoeken,et al.  Identification as a Mechanism of Narrative Persuasion , 2012, Commun. Res..

[8]  Linda R. Tropp,et al.  Interventions to reduce prejudice and enhance inclusion and respect for ethnic differences in early childhood: A systematic review , 2012 .

[9]  Geoff F. Kaufman,et al.  Changing beliefs and behavior through experience-taking. , 2012, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[10]  Ian A Apperly,et al.  Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. , 2012, Child development.

[11]  Sung-Yeon Park Mediated Intergroup Contact: Concept Explication, Synthesis, and Application , 2012 .

[12]  A. Beelmann,et al.  Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: a multinational meta-analysis of age differences. , 2011, Child development.

[13]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Preschoolers' quarantining of fantasy stories. , 2011, Child development.

[14]  A. Galinsky,et al.  Perspective taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  S. Lazic,et al.  Model based Inference in the Life Sciences: a Primer on Evidence , 2011 .

[16]  K. Williams,et al.  The Effects of Homophily, Identification, and Violent Video Games on Players , 2010 .

[17]  N. Ambady,et al.  Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development. , 2010, Child development.

[18]  D. O'Neill,et al.  A step at a time: Preliterate children’s simulation of narrative movement during story comprehension , 2010, Cognition.

[19]  Nurit Tal-Or,et al.  Understanding audience involvement: Conceptualizing and manipulating identification and transportation , 2010 .

[20]  Katherine D. Kinzler,et al.  Accent trumps race in guiding children's social preferences. , 2009, Social cognition.

[21]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Reading Stories Activates Neural Representations of Visual and Motor Experiences , 2009, Psychological science.

[22]  Gillian Ku,et al.  Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Emily Moyer-Gusé Toward a Theory of Entertainment Persuasion: Explaining the Persuasive Effects of Entertainment‐Education Messages , 2008 .

[24]  Lisa K. Fazio,et al.  Older, not younger, children learn more false facts from stories , 2008, Cognition.

[25]  J. Harwood,et al.  A Social Cognitive Theory Approach to the Effects of Mediated Intergroup Contact on Intergroup Attitudes , 2007 .

[26]  D. O'Neill,et al.  The emergence of the ability to track a character's mental perspective in narrative. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[27]  Kim Witte,et al.  Drama Theory and Entertainment Education: Exploring the Effects of a Radio Drama on Behavioral Intentions to Limit HIV Transmission in Ethiopia , 2007 .

[28]  E. E. Conger,et al.  Interaction and Participation for Young Hispanic and Caucasian Girls' and Boys' Learning of Media Content , 2007 .

[29]  Michelle R. Broaddus,et al.  Gender Stereotyping and Under-representation of Female Characters in 200 Popular Children’s Picture Books: A Twenty-first Century Update , 2006 .

[30]  Lindsey Cameron,et al.  Extended contact through story reading in school : Reducing children's prejudice toward the disabled , 2006 .

[31]  Gabrielle A. Strouse,et al.  Empathy for adolescents' role model selection and learning of DVD content , 2006 .

[32]  Matthew Lombard,et al.  At the Heart of It All: The Concept of Presence , 2006 .

[33]  Dean E. Hewes,et al.  The Parasocial Contact Hypothesis , 2005 .

[34]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction , 2004 .

[35]  E. S. Knowles,et al.  Narrative Persuasion and Overcoming Resistance , 2004 .

[36]  Albert Bandura,et al.  Social Cognitive Theory for Personal and Social Change by Enabling Media , 2003 .

[37]  A. Persson,et al.  The impact of a prejudice-prevention television program on young children’s ideas about race , 2003 .

[38]  Henry L. Roediger,et al.  Learning facts from fiction. , 2003 .

[39]  Lisa Shochat Our neighborhood: Using entertaining children's television to promote interethnic understanding in Macedonia , 2003 .

[40]  L. S. Liben,et al.  Race and the workforce: occupational status, aspirations, and stereotyping among African American children. , 2003, Developmental psychology.

[41]  A. Rubin,et al.  Viewer Aggression and Homophily, Identification, and Parasocial Relationships With Television Characters , 2003 .

[42]  C. Batson,et al.  Empathy, Attitudes, and Action: Can Feeling for a Member of a Stigmatized Group Motivate One to Help the Group? , 2002 .

[43]  D. Giles Parasocial Interaction: A Review of the Literature and a Model for Future Research , 2002 .

[44]  Timothy J. Bergen The Development of Prejudice in Children , 2001 .

[45]  Jonathan Cohen Defining Identification: A Theoretical Look at the Identification of Audiences With Media Characters , 2001 .

[46]  P. Harris,et al.  In Cinderella's slippers? Story comprehension from the protagonist's point of view. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[47]  Marjorie Taylor,et al.  Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them , 1999 .

[48]  Y. Fujioka Television Portrayals and African-American Stereotypes: Examination of Television Effects when Direct Contact is Lacking , 1999 .

[49]  Stephen C. Wright,et al.  The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. , 1997 .

[50]  B. Pescosolido,et al.  Culture and conflict : The portrayal of Blacks in U.S. children's picture books through the mid- and late-twentieth century , 1997 .

[51]  C. Hoffner Children's wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters , 1996 .

[52]  F. Aboud,et al.  A Longitudinal Study of White Children's Racial Prejudice as a Social-Cognitive Development. , 1995 .

[53]  J. Blair,et al.  Social Learning Theory , 1993, AAOHN journal : official journal of the American Association of Occupational Health Nurses.

[54]  G. Bower,et al.  Mental models in narrative comprehension. , 1990, Science.

[55]  G. Clunies-Ross,et al.  Changing the attitudes of students towards peers with disabilities , 1989 .

[56]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension , 1987 .

[57]  R. Gordon Folk Psychology as Simulation , 1986 .

[58]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[59]  William F. Brewer,et al.  Development of story liking: Character identification, suspense, and outcome resolution. , 1984 .

[60]  John B. Black,et al.  Point of view in narrative comprehension, memory, and production , 1979 .

[61]  Gordon H. Bower,et al.  Experiments on story comprehension and recall , 1978 .

[62]  P. Katz,et al.  Influence of Belief, Race, and Sex Similarities between Child Observers and Models on Attitudes and Observational Learning. , 1977 .

[63]  M. Hoffman,et al.  Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. , 1977, Psychological bulletin.

[64]  Luberta Mays On Meeting Real People: An Evaluation Report on Vegetable Soup: The Effects of a Multi-Ethnic Children's Television Series on Intergroup Attitudes of Children. , 1975 .

[65]  D. Best,et al.  The Measurement of Children's Racial Attitudes in the Early School Years. , 1975 .

[66]  D. Best,et al.  Preschool Racial Attitude Measure II , 1975 .

[67]  L. Furby,et al.  Racial Preferences in Young Children. , 1971 .

[68]  M. Rosekrans Imitation in children as a function of perceived similarity to a social model and vicarious reinforcement. , 1967, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[69]  J Tudor-Hart,et al.  On the nature of prejudice. , 1961, The Eugenics review.

[70]  Robert Liebendorfer Mind, self and society , 1960 .

[71]  E. Maccoby,et al.  Identification and observational learning from films. , 1957, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[72]  Kenneth B. Clark,et al.  Emotional Factors in Racial Identification and Preference in Negro Children , 1950 .

[73]  M. Goodman,et al.  Evidence concerning the genesis of interracial attitudes. , 1946, American anthropologist.

[74]  Caren M. Walker,et al.  Learning to learn from stories: children's developing sensitivity to the causal structure of fictional worlds. , 2015, Child development.

[75]  A. D. Graaf The Effectiveness of Adaptation of the Protagonist in Narrative Impact: Similarity Influences Health Beliefs through Self-Referencing , 2014 .

[76]  A. Goldman,et al.  Simulation theory. , 2010, Wiley interdisciplinary reviews. Cognitive science.

[77]  Peter Fischer,et al.  Media violence and the self: The impact of personalized gaming characters in aggressive video games on aggressive behavior , 2010 .

[78]  D. Green,et al.  Prejudice reduction: what works? A review and assessment of research and practice. , 2009, Annual review of psychology.

[79]  C. Meyer Role play. , 2009, Minnesota medicine.

[80]  Fenja Ziegler,et al.  How does narrative cue children's perspective taking? , 2005, Developmental psychology.

[81]  J. Flavell,et al.  Cognitive development: children's knowledge about the mind. , 1999, Annual review of psychology.

[82]  E. Harmon-Jones,et al.  Empathy and attitudes: can feeling for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group? , 1997, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[83]  Mary Ann Wham Enhancing Multicultural Awareness through the Storybook Reading Experience. , 1996 .

[84]  M. Lepper,et al.  Intrinsic motivation and the process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, personalization, and choice. , 1996 .

[85]  D. Wong,et al.  Pain in children: comparison of assessment scales. , 1988, The Oklahoma nurse.

[86]  B. Gunter Television and sex role stereotyping , 1986 .

[87]  D. A. Kenny,et al.  The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. , 1986, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[88]  D. Fernie Ordinary and extraordinary people: Children's understanding of television and real life models , 1981 .

[89]  Mark H. Davis,et al.  A Multidimensional Approach to Individual Differences in Empathy , 1980 .

[90]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Television's impact on preferences for non‐white playmates: Canadian “Sesame Street” inserts , 1979 .

[91]  B. Houser An examination of the use of audiovisual media in reducing prejudice , 1978 .

[92]  T. Brock,et al.  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology the Role of Transportation in the Persuasiveness of Public Narratives Text Quality Individual Differences and Situational Influences Transportation Scale Items Gender Differences Discriminant Validation: Need for Cognition Effect of Text Manipulation Beli , 2022 .

[93]  Lindsey Cameron,et al.  Changing Children’s Intergroup Attitudes Toward Refugees: Testing Different Models of Extended Contact , 2022 .

[94]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article Journal of Cognition and Development Learning from Fantasy and Real Characters in Preschool and Kindergarten , 2022 .