Iterative Bayesian Estimation as an Explanation for Range and Regression Effects: A Study on Human Path Integration

Systematic errors in human path integration were previously associated with processing deficits in the integration of space and time. In the present work, we hypothesized that these errors are de facto the result of a system that aims to optimize its performance by incorporating knowledge about prior experience into the current estimate of displacement. We tested human linear and angular displacement estimation behavior in a production–reproduction task under three different prior experience conditions where samples were drawn from different overlapping sample distributions. We found that (1) behavior was biased toward the center of the underlying sample distribution, (2) the amount of bias increased with increasing sample range, and (3) the standard deviation for all conditions was linearly dependent on the mean reproduced displacements. We propose a model of Bayesian estimation on logarithmic scales that explains the observed behavior by optimal fusion of an experience-dependent prior expectation with the current noisy displacement measurement. The iterative update of prior experience is modeled by the formulation of a discrete Kalman filter. The model provides a direct link between Weber–Fechner and Stevens' power law, providing a mechanistic explanation for universal psychophysical effects in human magnitude estimation such as the regression to the mean and the range effect.

[1]  S. S. Stevens On the psychophysical law. , 1957, Psychological review.

[2]  S S Stevens,et al.  To Honor Fechner and Repeal His Law: A power function, not a log function, describes the operating characteristic of a sensory system. , 1961, Science.

[3]  D. M. MacKay,et al.  Psychophysics of Perceived Intensity: A Theoretical Basis for Fechner's and Stevens' Laws , 1963, Science.

[4]  H. Eisler,et al.  ON THE PROBLEM OF HYSTERESIS IN PSYCHOPHYSICS. , 1963 .

[5]  S. S. Stevens,et al.  Regression effect in psychophysical judgment , 1966 .

[6]  S. S. Stevens Issues in psychophysical measurement. , 1971 .

[7]  M. Teghtsoonian,et al.  Range and regression effects in magnitude scaling , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[8]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Acquisition of route and survey knowledge in the absence of vision. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.

[9]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Idiothetic navigation in Gerbils and Humans , 1991 .

[10]  R. Klatzky,et al.  Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted: assessment of path integration ability. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[11]  Kathleen H. Kowal The range effect as a function of stimulus set, presence of a standard, and modulus , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  J M Zanker,et al.  Does Motion Perception Follow Weber's Law? , 1995, Perception.

[13]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Spatial orientation in humans: perception of angular whole-body displacements in two-dimensional trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  G. Fechner Elemente der Psychophysik , 1998 .

[15]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[16]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  Observer biases in the 3D interpretation of line drawings , 1998, Vision Research.

[17]  W. Becker,et al.  Estimation of self-turning in the dark: comparison between active and passive rotation , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[18]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Scale-invariance as a unifying psychological principle , 1999, Cognition.

[19]  D. Laming Prior expectations in cross-modality matching , 1999 .

[20]  M. Schwartz Haptic perception of the distance walked when blindfolded. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[22]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Visual Homing Is Possible Without Landmarks: A Path Integration Study in Virtual Reality , 2002, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[24]  Vincent Walsh A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[25]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[26]  D. Knill,et al.  The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[27]  Ariane S Etienne,et al.  Path integration in mammals , 2004, Hippocampus.

[28]  M. Ernst,et al.  Experience can change the 'light-from-above' prior , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[29]  H. Mittelstaedt,et al.  Homing by path integration in a mammal , 1980, Naturwissenschaften.

[30]  Alexander Thiele,et al.  Effects on orientation perception of manipulating the spatio–temporal prior probability of stimuli , 2004, Vision Research.

[31]  Konrad Paul Körding,et al.  The loss function of sensorimotor learning. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Bayesian integration in force estimation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[34]  Howard S Hock,et al.  Dynamical vs. judgmental comparison: hysteresis effects in motion perception. , 2005, Spatial vision.

[35]  Markus Lappe,et al.  Absolute travel distance from optic flow , 2005, Vision Research.

[36]  M. Miyazaki,et al.  Testing Bayesian models of human coincidence timing. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  Michael S Landy,et al.  Combining Priors and Noisy Visual Cues in a Rapid Pointing Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Bayesian brain : probabilistic approaches to neural coding , 2006 .

[39]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  W. Becker,et al.  Perception of angular displacement without landmarks: evidence for Bayesian fusion of vestibular, optokinetic, podokinesthetic, and cognitive information , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[41]  Laurence R. Harris,et al.  Travel distance estimation from visual motion by leaky path integration , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  Andreas Nieder,et al.  A Labeled-Line Code for Small and Large Numerosities in the Monkey Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Vestibular perception and navigation in the congenitally blind. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Y. Ivanenko,et al.  Space-time relativity in self-motion reproduction. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[45]  M. Ernst,et al.  The statistical determinants of adaptation rate in human reaching. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[46]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The precision of locomotor odometry in humans , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  Nava Rubin,et al.  Bi-stable depth ordering of superimposed moving gratings. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[48]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Traveled distances: New insights into the role of optic flow , 2008, Vision Research.

[49]  Pierre Pica,et al.  Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures , 2008, Science.

[50]  Christopher R Fetsch,et al.  Dynamic Reweighting of Visual and Vestibular Cues during Self-Motion Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  R. J. Beers,et al.  Motor Learning Is Optimally Tuned to the Properties of Motor Noise , 2009, Neuron.

[52]  Heiko Hecht,et al.  Distance estimation in vista space , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[53]  Melissa E. Libertus,et al.  Comment on "Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures" , 2009, Science.

[54]  Christine J. Ziemer,et al.  Estimating distance in real and virtual environments: Does order make a difference? , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[55]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  The Effect of Dual Tasks in Locomotor Path Integration , 2009, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[56]  On the origin of systematic errors in a simple navigation task , 2009, BMC Neuroscience.

[57]  Ken Cheng,et al.  Categories and Range Effects in Human Spatial Memory , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[58]  Reginald G. Golledge,et al.  The Encoding-Error Model of Pathway Completion without Vision , 2010 .

[59]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Learning Priors for Bayesian Computations in the Nervous System , 2010, PloS one.

[60]  Michael N. Shadlen,et al.  Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[61]  Jennifer L. Campos,et al.  Bayesian integration of visual and vestibular signals for heading. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[62]  Heiko Hecht,et al.  Locomotor and verbal distance judgments in action and vista space , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  F. Urban The Central Tendency of Judgment. , 2022 .