Inhibition deficit in the spatial tendency of the response in multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. An event-related potential study

Longitudinal studies have shown that a high percentage of people with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) develop Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Prodromal AD is known to involve deficits in executive control processes. In the present study, we examined such deficits by recording EEG in 13 single-domain amnestic MCI (sdaMCI), 12 multiple-domain amnestic MCI (mdaMCI) and 18 healthy elderly (control group, CG) participants while they performed a Simon task. The Simon task demands deployment of executive processes because participants have to respond to non-spatial features of a lateralized stimulus and inhibit the more automatic spatial tendency of the response. We specifically focused on the negativity central contralateral (N2cc), an event-related potential (ERP) component related to brain activity that prevents the cross-talk between direction of spatial attention and manual response preparation. The reaction time (RT) was not significantly different among the three groups of participants. The percentage of errors (PE) was higher in mdaMCI than in CG and sdaMCI participants. In addition, N2cc latency was delayed in mdaMCI (i.e., delayed implementation of mechanisms for controlling the spatial tendency of the response). The N2cc latency clearly distinguished among mdaMCI and CG/sdaMCI participants (area under curve: 0.91). Longer N2cc was therefore associated with executive control deficits, which suggests that N2cc latency is a correlate of mdaMCI.

[1]  R. Petersen,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment , 2006, The Lancet.

[2]  H. Chertkow,et al.  The Profile of Executive Functioning in Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment: Disproportionate Deficits in Inhibitory Control , 2012, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[3]  C. Jack,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment – beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2004, Journal of internal medicine.

[4]  Oscar L. Lopez,et al.  Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of executive control in very old individuals with mild cognitive impairment , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[5]  S. Luck,et al.  Bridging the Gap between Monkey Neurophysiology and Human Perception: An Ambiguity Resolution Theory of Visual Selective Attention , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[6]  A. Ragazzoni,et al.  Atypical event-related potentials in patients with mild cognitive impairment: An identification-priming study , 2010, Alzheimer's & Dementia.

[7]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning , 2004, Brain and Cognition.

[8]  María Jesús Benedet,et al.  TAVEC: test de aprendizaje verbal España-Complutense : manual , 1998 .

[9]  M. Eimer The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. , 1996, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[10]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  The Simon effect in cognitive electrophysiology: A short review. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[11]  M. Lawton,et al.  Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. , 1969, The Gerontologist.

[12]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Inefficient response inhibition in individuals with mild cognitive impairment , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  S. Kornblum,et al.  A Parallel Distributed Processing Model of Stimulus–Stimulus and Stimulus–Response Compatibility , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[14]  Vincenzo Paolo Senese,et al.  Visuospatial memory in healthy elderly, AD and MCI: a review. , 2009, Current aging science.

[15]  Kyle Steenland,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment: an opportunity to identify patients at high risk for progression to Alzheimer's disease. , 2006, Clinical therapeutics.

[16]  C. Babiloni,et al.  Conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease is predicted by sources and coherence of brain electroencephalography rhythms , 2006, Neuroscience.

[17]  F. Díaz,et al.  Effects of amnestic mild cognitive impairment on N2 and P3 Go/NoGo ERP components. , 2013, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[18]  D. Facal,et al.  Does tip-of-the-tongue for proper names discriminate amnestic mild cognitive impairment? , 2012, International Psychogeriatrics.

[19]  H. Leuthold,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence for response priming and conflict regulation in the auditory Simon task , 2006, Brain Research.

[20]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  J. Yesavage,et al.  Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. , 1986 .

[22]  J. Ashe,et al.  The Effect of Stimulus–Response Compatibility on Cortical Motor Activation , 2001, NeuroImage.

[23]  F. Díaz,et al.  Electrophysiological Correlates of Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment in a Simon Task , 2013, PloS one.

[24]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Prior information in motor and premotor cortex: activity during the delay period and effect on pre-movement activity. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  P. Praamstra,et al.  Failed Suppression of Direct Visuomotor Activation in Parkinson's Disease , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[27]  Antonio Lobo,et al.  Revalidación y normalización del Mini-Examen Cognoscitivo (primera versión en castellano del Mini-Mental Status Examination) en la población general geriátrica , 1999 .

[28]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Mild Cognitive Impairment Clinical Characterization and Outcome , 1999 .

[29]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  Seishi Terada,et al.  [Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS)]. , 2011, Nihon rinsho. Japanese journal of clinical medicine.

[31]  D. Facal,et al.  Cognitive interference and aging: insights from a spatial stimulus-response consistency task. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[32]  D. Delis,et al.  The California verbal learning test , 2016 .

[33]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility: An Integrated Perspective , 1990 .

[34]  Martin Eimer,et al.  The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[35]  R. Petersen,et al.  Mild Cognitive Impairment: An Overview , 2008, CNS Spectrums.

[36]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  F. Díaz,et al.  The Simon effect modulates N2cc and LRP but not the N2pc component. , 2012, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[38]  Robert W. Proctor,et al.  Aging and Response Selection in Spatial Choice Tasks , 2005, Hum. Factors.

[39]  S P Wise,et al.  The premotor cortex and nonstandard sensorimotor mapping. , 1996, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[40]  W. Krampla,et al.  Conversion from subtypes of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer dementia , 2007, Neurology.

[41]  G. Barrett Clinical application of event-related potentials in dementing illness: issues and problems. , 2000, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[42]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Excessive sub-threshold motor preparation for non-target stimuli in normal aging , 2010, NeuroImage.

[43]  F. Díaz,et al.  Age-related changes in ERP correlates of visuospatial and motor processes. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[44]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  Hélène Amieva,et al.  Inhibitory functioning in Alzheimer's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Geoffrey F. Woodman,et al.  Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search , 1999, Nature.

[47]  R. Petersen Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity , 2004, Journal of internal medicine.

[48]  Sherry A. Greenberg,et al.  Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). , 2007, Occasional paper.

[49]  C. Jack,et al.  Mild cognitive impairment: ten years later. , 2009, Archives of neurology.

[50]  Keith A Hutchison,et al.  Spatial attention and response control in healthy younger and older adults and individuals with Alzheimer's disease: evidence for disproportionate selection impairments in the Simon task. , 2007, Neuropsychology.

[51]  E Donchin,et al.  A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[52]  Ravi S. Menon,et al.  A comparison of frontoparietal fMRI activation during anti-saccades and anti-pointing. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  Montserrat Zurrón,et al.  Mismatch negativity (MMN) amplitude as a biomarker of sensory memory deficit in amnestic mild cognitive impairment , 2013, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[54]  T. Chartrand,et al.  The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. , 2000 .

[55]  P. Pazo-Álvarez,et al.  Response processing during visual search in normal aging: The need for more time to prevent cross talk between spatial attention and manual response selection , 2012, Biological Psychology.

[56]  S. Luck,et al.  Neural sources of focused attention in visual search. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  Prior information of stimulus location: Effects on ERP measures of visual selection and response selection , 2006, Brain Research.

[58]  Paul B. Johnson,et al.  Premotor and parietal cortex: corticocortical connectivity and combinatorial computations. , 1997, Annual review of neuroscience.

[59]  A van Oosterom,et al.  Overlap of attention and movement-related activity in lateralized event-related brain potentials , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[60]  E. Tangalos,et al.  Mortality in amnestic mild cognitive impairment , 2006, Neurology.

[61]  G. Dreisbach Current Directions in Psychological Science Gesine Dreisbach Mechanisms of Cognitive Control : The Functional Role of Task Rules , 2012 .

[62]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Attention and movement-related motor cortex activation: a high-density EEG study of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[63]  F. Huppert,et al.  CAMDEX: A Standardised Instrument for the Diagnosis of Mental Disorder in the Elderly with Special Reference to the Early Detection of Dementia , 1986, British Journal of Psychiatry.

[64]  Juliana Yordanova,et al.  Motor-response generation as a source of aging-related behavioural slowing in choice-reaction tasks , 2006, Neurobiology of Aging.

[65]  A. Lobo,et al.  [Revalidation and standardization of the cognition mini-exam (first Spanish version of the Mini-Mental Status Examination) in the general geriatric population]. , 1999, Medicina clinica.

[66]  R. Verleger,et al.  Aging and the Simon task. , 2002, Psychophysiology.

[67]  Vincent Di Lollo,et al.  Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[68]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[69]  R. V. D. Van der Lubbe,et al.  Endogenous orienting modulates the Simon effect: critical factors in experimental design , 2007, Psychological research.

[70]  S. Wise,et al.  The premotor cortex and nonstandard sensorimotor mapping 1 , 1996 .

[71]  J. Hodges,et al.  Attention and executive deficits in Alzheimer's disease. A critical review. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.