Uncertainty and confidence from the triple-network perspective: Voxel-based meta-analyses

Our subjective confidence about particular events is related to but independent from the objective certainty of the stimuli we encounter. Surprisingly, previous investigations of the neurophysiological correlates of confidence and uncertainty have largely been carried out separately. After systematically reviewing the blood oxygenation-level dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging (BOLD fMRI) literature, and splitting studies on the basis of their task requirements, a voxel-based meta-analysis was performed to identify: (i) those regions which are replicably modulated by the uncertainty of environmental conditions; (ii) those regions whose activity is robustly affected by our subjective confidence; and (iii) those regions differentially activated at these contrasting times. In further meta-analyses the consistency of activation between these judgement types was assessed. Increased activation was consistently observed in the salience (anterior cingulate cortex and insula) and central executive network (dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices) in conditions of increased uncertainty; by contrast, default mode network (midline cortical and medial temporal lobe) regions robustly exhibited a positive relationship with subjective confidence. Regions including right parahippocampal gyrus were positively modulated by magnitude across both certainty and confidence judgements. This region was also shown to be more significantly modulated by confidence magnitude as compared with degree of environmental certainty. The functional and methodological implications of these findings are discussed with a view to improving future investigation of the neural basis of metacognitive judgement.

[1]  Guillén Fernández,et al.  Subjective Sense of Memory Strength and the Objective Amount of Information Accurately Remembered Are Related to Distinct Neural Correlates at Encoding , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Neural Correlates of Metamemory: A Comparison of Feeling-of-Knowing and Retrospective Confidence Judgments , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  Scott M. Hayes,et al.  Neural Correlates of Confidence during Item Recognition and Source Memory Retrieval: Evidence for Both Dual-process and Strength Memory Theories , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[5]  Issidoros C. Sarinopoulos,et al.  Uncertainty during anticipation modulates neural responses to aversion in human insula and amygdala. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[6]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Measuring consciousness: Is one measure better than the other? , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[7]  Steffen Moritz,et al.  A generalized bias against disconfirmatory evidence in schizophrenia , 2006, Psychiatry Research.

[8]  G. Glover,et al.  Error‐related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task , 2001, Human brain mapping.

[9]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[10]  T. Shallice,et al.  Confidence in Recognition Memory for Words: Dissociating Right Prefrontal Roles in Episodic Retrieval , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Understanding metamemory: Neural correlates of the cognitive process and subjective level of confidence in recognition memory , 2006, NeuroImage.

[12]  R. Dolan,et al.  Metacognition: computation, biology and function , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[13]  R. Dolan,et al.  The neural basis of metacognitive ability , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[14]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Higher order thoughts in action: consciousness as an unconscious re-description process , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Erroneous analyses of interactions in neuroscience: a problem of significance , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[17]  Rajesh P. N. Rao,et al.  Bayesian brain : probabilistic approaches to neural coding , 2006 .

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Acute Changes in Frontoparietal Activity after Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation over the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in a Cued Reaction Time Task , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Why am I unsure? Internal and external attributions of uncertainty dissociated by fMRI , 2004, NeuroImage.

[20]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Neural Response to Reward Anticipation under Risk Is Nonlinear in Probabilities , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  Peter F. Liddle,et al.  Structural correlates of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis , 2012, Schizophrenia Research.

[22]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  H. Critchley,et al.  Neural Activity in the Human Brain Relating to Uncertainty and Arousal during Anticipation , 2001, Neuron.

[24]  Z. Dienes,et al.  Gambling on the unconscious: A comparison of wagering and confidence ratings as measures of awareness in an artificial grammar task , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[25]  Martin Strobel,et al.  To trust or not to trust: the dynamics of social interaction in psychosis. , 2012, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[26]  Stephanie Spengler,et al.  Differential functions of lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10) revealed by brain-behavior associations. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  Carlo Miniussi,et al.  The role of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in visual change awareness , 2004, Neuroreport.

[28]  O. Howes,et al.  Mapping vulnerability to bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. , 2012, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.

[29]  Matthew J. Kempton,et al.  Neuroanatomy of vulnerability to psychosis: A voxel-based meta-analysis , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[30]  S. Zysset,et al.  Dopaminergic modulation of brain systems subserving decision making under uncertainty: A study with fMRI and methylphenidate challenge , 2009, Synapse.

[31]  R. Passingham,et al.  Relative blindsight in normal observers and the neural correlate of visual consciousness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Optimizing subjective measures of consciousness , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[33]  Heinrich Sauer,et al.  The neural correlates of reward-related trial-and-error learning: an fMRI study with a probabilistic learning task. , 2008, Learning & memory.

[34]  G H Glover,et al.  Image‐based method for retrospective correction of physiological motion effects in fMRI: RETROICOR , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[35]  J Radua,et al.  A new meta-analytic method for neuroimaging studies that combines reported peak coordinates and statistical parametric maps , 2012, European Psychiatry.

[36]  R. Raedt,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and Stroop performance: Tackling the lateralization , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[37]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Predicting events of varying probability: uncertainty investigated by fMRI , 2003, NeuroImage.

[38]  V. Menon Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[39]  Marian Stamp Dawkins,et al.  The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of Brain Function The Noisy Brain: Stochastic Dynamics as a Principle of Brain Function. By Edmund T. Rolls & Gustavo Deco. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2010). Pp. 310. Price £37.95 hardback. , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[40]  V. Giampietro,et al.  Default distrust? An fMRI investigation of the neural development of trust and cooperation. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[41]  J. Raduà,et al.  Voxel-wise meta-analysis of grey matter changes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. , 2009, The British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science.

[42]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[43]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Dissociating Confidence and Accuracy: Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Shows Origins of the Subjective Memory Experience , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[44]  V. Menon,et al.  A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[45]  Paul C. Fletcher,et al.  Separable Forms of Reality Monitoring Supported by Anterior Prefrontal Cortex , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[46]  J. A. Pruszynski,et al.  Neural correlates , 2023 .

[47]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[48]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Decisions under Uncertainty: Probabilistic Context Influences Activation of Prefrontal and Parietal Cortices , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  J. Hirsch,et al.  A Neural Representation of Categorization Uncertainty in the Human Brain , 2006, Neuron.

[50]  I. Olkin,et al.  Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology - A proposal for reporting , 2000 .

[51]  C. Frith Social cognition , 2008, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[52]  Zhiye Chen,et al.  Grey matter volume changes over the whole brain in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: A voxel-wise meta-analysis of voxel based morphometry studies , 2010, Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis : official publication of the World Federation of Neurology Research Group on Motor Neuron Diseases.

[53]  H. Lau,et al.  A detection theoretic explanation of blindsight suggests a link between conscious perception and metacognition , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[54]  Peter Carruthers,et al.  How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition , 2009, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[55]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[56]  M. Koivisto,et al.  The electrophysiology of introspection , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[57]  Hongkeun Kim,et al.  Trusting Our Memories: Dissociating the Neural Correlates of Confidence in Veridical versus Illusory Memories , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  F. Mauguière,et al.  Parietal and cingulate processes in central pain. A combined positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study of an unusual case , 2000, Pain.

[59]  Adam Kepecs,et al.  A computational framework for the study of confidence in humans and animals , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[60]  Christian Büchel,et al.  Neural correlates of memory confidence , 2006, NeuroImage.

[61]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Prefrontal, Parietal, and Temporal Cortex Networks Underlie Decision-Making in the Presence of Uncertainty , 2001, NeuroImage.

[62]  ChrisD . Frith,et al.  Perceiving is believing: a Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[63]  Hongkeun Kim,et al.  Common and specific brain regions in high- versus low-confidence recognition memory , 2009, Brain Research.

[64]  Kristian Sandberg,et al.  Kinds of access: different methods for report reveal different kinds of metacognitive access , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[65]  S. Francis,et al.  Theta power during encoding predicts subsequent‐memory performance and default mode network deactivation , 2013, Human brain mapping.