A neural network model of semantic memory linking feature-based object representation and words

Recent theories in cognitive neuroscience suggest that semantic memory is a distributed process, which involves many cortical areas and is based on a multimodal representation of objects. The aim of this work is to extend a previous model of object representation to realize a semantic memory, in which sensory-motor representations of objects are linked with words. The model assumes that each object is described as a collection of features, coded in different cortical areas via a topological organization. Features in different objects are segmented via gamma-band synchronization of neural oscillators. The feature areas are further connected with a lexical area, devoted to the representation of words. Synapses among the feature areas, and among the lexical area and the feature areas are trained via a time-dependent Hebbian rule, during a period in which individual objects are presented together with the corresponding words. Simulation results demonstrate that, during the retrieval phase, the network can deal with the simultaneous presence of objects (from sensory-motor inputs) and words (from acoustic inputs), can correctly associate objects with words and segment objects even in the presence of incomplete information. Moreover, the network can realize some semantic links among words representing objects with shared features. These results support the idea that semantic memory can be described as an integrated process, whose content is retrieved by the co-activation of different multimodal regions. In perspective, extended versions of this model may be used to test conceptual theories, and to provide a quantitative assessment of existing data (for instance concerning patients with neural deficits).

[1]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  S. Grossberg The Link between Brain Learning, Attention, and Consciousness , 1999, Consciousness and Cognition.

[3]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Reading cinnamon activates olfactory brain regions , 2006, NeuroImage.

[4]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Hierarchies, similarity, and interactivity in object recognition: “Category-specific” neuropsychological deficits , 2001, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  N. Birbaumer,et al.  Brain Rhythms of Language: Nouns Versus Verbs , 1996, The European journal of neuroscience.

[6]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The multiple semantics hypothesis: Multiple confusions? , 1990 .

[7]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Theta and Gamma Oscillations Predict Encoding and Retrieval of Declarative Memory , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  S L Small,et al.  Distributed representations of semantic knowledge in the brain. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[9]  M. Ursino,et al.  Object segmentation and recovery via neural oscillators implementing the similarity and prior knowledge gestalt rules. , 2006, Bio Systems.

[10]  W. Marslen-Wilson Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition , 1987, Cognition.

[11]  W Singer,et al.  Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  M. Kraut,et al.  The Semantic Object Retrieval Test (SORT) in Normal Aging and Alzheimer Disease , 2006, Cognitive and behavioral neurology : official journal of the Society for Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology.

[13]  G. Gainotti Anatomical functional and cognitive determinants of semantic memory disorders , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[14]  L. Abbott,et al.  Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  Mauro Ursino,et al.  Recognition of Abstract Objects Via Neural Oscillators: Interaction Among Topological Organization, Associative Memory and Gamma Band Synchronization , 2009, IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks.

[16]  Sergio E. Chaigneau,et al.  THE SIMILARITY-IN-TOPOGRAPHY PRINCIPLE: RECONCILING THEORIES OF CONCEPTUAL DEFICITS , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[17]  A. Pérez-Villalba Rhythms of the Brain, G. Buzsáki. Oxford University Press, Madison Avenue, New York (2006), Price: GB £42.00, p. 448, ISBN: 0-19-530106-4 , 2008 .

[18]  Ch. von der Malsburg,et al.  A neural cocktail-party processor , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[19]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Walking or Talking?: Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Action Verb Processing , 2001, Brain and Language.

[20]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[21]  J. Ward,et al.  The Student's Guide to Cognitive Neuroscience , 2006 .

[22]  R. Traub,et al.  Neuronal networks for induced ‘40 Hz’ rhythms , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  W. Singer,et al.  Temporal binding and the neural correlates of sensory awareness , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  T. Rogers,et al.  Neural basis of category-specific semantic deficits for living things: evidence from semantic dementia, HSVE and a neural network model. , 2006, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Kraut,et al.  Object Activation from Features in the Semantic System , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Structure and deterioration of semantic memory: a neuropsychological and computational investigation. , 2004, Psychological review.

[27]  P. Milner A model for visual shape recognition. , 1974, Psychological review.

[28]  Gail Tillman,et al.  Neural substrates of semantic memory , 2007, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[29]  Guido Gainotti,et al.  What the Locus of Brain Lesion Tells us About the Nature of the Cognitive Defect Underlying Category-Specific Disorders: A Review , 2000, Cortex.

[30]  L. Tyler,et al.  Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[31]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Functional links between motor and language systems , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[32]  Tamer Demiralp,et al.  The phase‐locking of auditory gamma band responses in humans is sensitive to task processing , 1997, Neuroreport.

[33]  W. Marslen-Wilson,et al.  The temporal structure of spoken language understanding , 1980, Cognition.

[34]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[35]  L. K. Tyler,et al.  Conceptual Structure and the Structure of Concepts: A Distributed Account of Category-Specific Deficits , 2000, Brain and Language.

[36]  J J Hopfield,et al.  What is a moment? Transient synchrony as a collective mechanism for spatiotemporal integration. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  H. Markram,et al.  Regulation of Synaptic Efficacy by Coincidence of Postsynaptic APs and EPSPs , 1997, Science.

[38]  Michael A. Kraut,et al.  Neural Substrates of Semantics , 2002 .

[39]  Reinhard Eckhorn,et al.  Neural mechanisms of scene segmentation: recordings from the visual cortex suggest basic circuits for linking field models , 1999, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[40]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain Signatures of Meaning Access in Action Word Recognition , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  E. Warrington,et al.  THE SELECTIVE IMPAIRMENT OF FRUIT AND VEGETABLE KNOWLEDGE:AMULTIPLE PROCESSING CHANNELS ACCOUNT OF FINE-GRAIN CATEGORY SPECIFICITY , 2003, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[42]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[43]  A. Damasio Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[44]  F. Pulvermüller,et al.  Neurophysiological distinction of verb categories , 2000, Neuroreport.

[45]  A Morelli,et al.  Neural coding for the retrieval of multiple memory patterns. , 2006, Bio Systems.

[46]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[47]  E. Warrington,et al.  Category specific access dysphasia. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[48]  F. Varela,et al.  Perception's shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity , 1999, Nature.

[49]  M. A. Virasoro,et al.  THE COMPUTATIONAL ROLE OF CONSCIOUS PROCESSING IN A MODEL OF SEMANTIC MEMORY , 1997 .

[50]  W. Klimesch,et al.  What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes? , 2008, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[51]  P. Jonas,et al.  Synaptic mechanisms of synchronized gamma oscillations in inhibitory interneuron networks , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[52]  W. Singer,et al.  Synchronization of Neural Activity across Cortical Areas Correlates with Conscious Perception , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[54]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Domain-Specific Knowledge Systems in the Brain: The Animate-Inanimate Distinction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Category-Specific Semantic Deficits in Focal and Widespread Brain Damage: A Computational Account , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  D. Johnston,et al.  Regulation of Synaptic Efficacy by Coincidence of Postsynaptic APs and EPSPs , 1997 .

[57]  D. Neary,et al.  Knowledge of famous faces and names in semantic dementia. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Discrete Cortical Regions Associated with Knowledge of Color and Knowledge of Action , 1995, Science.

[59]  S. Bressler,et al.  Episodic multiregional cortical coherence at multiple frequencies during visual task performance , 1993, Nature.

[60]  DeLiang Wang,et al.  Image Segmentation Based on Oscillatory Correlation , 1997, Neural Computation.

[61]  E. Rolls,et al.  Cognition, Computation, and Consciousness , 1997 .