The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children

The dimensional change card sort (DCCS) is an easily administered and widely used measure of executive function that is suitable for use with participants across a wide range of ages. In the standard version, children are required to sort a series of bivalent test cards, first according to one dimension (e.g., color), and then according to the other (e.g., shape). Most 3-year-olds perseverate during the post-switch phase, exhibiting a pattern of inflexibility similar to that seen in patients with prefrontal cortical damage. By 5 years of age, most children switch when instructed to do so. Performance on the DCCS provides an index of the development of executive function, and it is impaired in children with disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism. We describe the protocol for the standard version (duration = 5 min) and the more challenging border version (duration = 5 min), which may be used with children as old as 7 years.*Note: In the version of the article originally published, the boat in the upper right of the figure was blue. It should be red. The error has been corrected in the HTML and PDF versions of the article.

[1]  W. Overton,et al.  The role of negative priming in preschoolers' flexible rule use on the dimensional change card sort task. , 2006, Child development.

[2]  Y. Munakata,et al.  All Together Now: When Dissociations Between Knowledge and Action Disappear , 2001, Psychological science.

[3]  F. Xavier Castellanos,et al.  Shifting-Related Brain Magnetic Activity in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder , 2006, Biological Psychiatry.

[4]  P. Zelazo The development of conscious control in childhood , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Douglas Frye,et al.  The relation between theory of mind and rule use: evidence from persons with autism‐spectrum disorders , 2002 .

[6]  P. Zelazo,et al.  An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them ☆ , 1996 .

[7]  W. Overton,et al.  The Development of Symbolic Coordination: Representation of Imagined Objects, Executive Function, and Theory of Mind , 2005 .

[8]  Ulrich Müller,et al.  The development of executive function in early childhood: I. The development of executive function. , 2003 .

[9]  E. Bialystok Cognitive Complexity and Attentional Control in the Bilingual Mind , 1999 .

[10]  L. Fenson,et al.  The Effects of Feedback on Perseverative Errors in Preschool Aged Children , 2005 .

[11]  Adele Diamond,et al.  Not Quite as Grown-Up as We Like to Think , 2005, Psychological science.

[12]  Douglas Frye,et al.  Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning , 1995 .

[13]  S. M. Carlson Developmentally Sensitive Measures of Executive Function in Preschool Children , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.

[14]  L. Moses,et al.  Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. , 2001, Child development.

[15]  Natasha Z. Kirkham,et al.  ARTICLE WITH PEER COMMENTARIES AND RESPONSE Helping children apply their knowledge to their behavior on a dimension-switching task , 2003 .

[16]  D. Custance,et al.  Rule‐based reasoning and theory of mind in autism: a commentary on the work of Zelazo, Jacques, Burack and Frye , 2002 .

[17]  Josef Perner,et al.  Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: helping children apply conflicting descriptions. , 2003, Child development.

[18]  Silvia A. Bunge,et al.  A Brain-Based Account of the Development of Rule Use in Childhood , 2006 .

[19]  B. Milner Effects of Different Brain Lesions on Card Sorting: The Role of the Frontal Lobes , 1963 .

[20]  F. Craik,et al.  Executive function across the life span. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[21]  Philip David Zelazo,et al.  Assessment of Hot and Cool Executive Function in Young Children: Age-Related Changes and Individual Differences , 2005, Developmental neuropsychology.