Divided attention impairs human motor adaptation but not feedback control.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Shadmehr,et al. Intact ability to learn internal models of arm dynamics in Huntington's disease but not cerebellar degeneration. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[2] Vijaykumar Gullapalli,et al. A stochastic reinforcement learning algorithm for learning real-valued functions , 1990, Neural Networks.
[3] Terence D. Sanger,et al. Failure of Motor Learning for Large Initial Errors , 2004, Neural Computation.
[4] Tim Curran,et al. Attentional and Nonattentional Forms of Sequence Learning , 1993 .
[5] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.
[6] Tracy L. Faber,et al. Role of posterior parietal cortex in the recalibration of visually guided reaching , 1996, Nature.
[7] R. Shadmehr,et al. Motor disorder in Huntington's disease begins as a dysfunction in error feedback control , 2000, Nature.
[8] T. Flash,et al. The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[9] R C Miall,et al. System Identification Applied to a Visuomotor Task: Near-Optimal Human Performance in a Noisy Changing Task , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[10] Rieko Osu,et al. The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance , 2001, Nature.
[11] Reza Shadmehr,et al. Quantifying Generalization from Trial-by-Trial Behavior of Adaptive Systems that Learn with Basis Functions: Theory and Experiments in Human Motor Control , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[12] R Shadmehr,et al. Spatial Generalization from Learning Dynamics of Reaching Movements , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[13] F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] Armando B. Corripio,et al. Principles and Practice of Automatic Process Control , 1985 .
[15] John H. J. Allum,et al. Responses to load disturbances in human shoulder muscles: The hypothesis that one component is a pulse test information signal , 1975, Experimental Brain Research.
[16] Tatsuya Kimura,et al. Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements , 1998, Nature.
[17] R. Shadmehr,et al. Neural correlates of motor memory consolidation. , 1997, Science.
[18] Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al. Motor adaptation to single force pulses: sensitive to direction but insensitive to within-movement pulse placement and magnitude. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.
[19] A. Savitzky,et al. Smoothing and Differentiation of Data by Simplified Least Squares Procedures. , 1964 .
[20] T. Brashers-Krug,et al. Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[21] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[22] Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Sequence, time, or state representation: how does the motor control system adapt to variable environments? , 2003, Biological Cybernetics.
[23] J. Lackner,et al. Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[24] G. M. Redding,et al. Cognitive Load and Prism Adaptation. , 1992, Journal of motor behavior.
[25] P. Matthews,et al. Distinguishable brain activation networks for short- and long-term motor skill learning. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.
[26] W. G. Koster,et al. The psychological refractory period , 1966 .
[27] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Role of the posterior parietal cortex in updating reaching movements to a visual target , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[28] D. Noll,et al. Bilateral basal ganglia activation associated with sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[29] C. D. MARSDEN,et al. Servo Action in Human Voluntary Movement , 1972, Nature.
[30] Scott T. Grafton,et al. A lesion of the posterior parietal cortex disrupts on-line adjustments during aiming movements , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[31] Andrew G. Barto,et al. Reinforcement learning in motor control , 1998 .
[32] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[33] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Functional Anatomy of Nonvisual Feedback Loops during Reaching: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[34] D. Wolpert,et al. Temporal and amplitude generalization in motor learning. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] M. Kawato,et al. A hierarchical neural-network model for control and learning of voluntary movement , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[36] Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al. Anatomy of motor learning. II. Subcortical structures and learning by trial and error. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.
[37] J. Gordon,et al. Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[38] R. D Seidler,et al. Feedforward and feedback processes in motor control , 2004, NeuroImage.
[39] J. Flanagan,et al. Task-specific internal models for kinematic transformations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[40] Teuvo Kohonen,et al. Where the abstract feature maps of the brain might come from , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.
[41] R L Sainburg,et al. Intersegmental dynamics are controlled by sequential anticipatory, error correction, and postural mechanisms. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Cerebellum and motor control , 1998 .
[43] D. Ostry,et al. Stimulation of the Posterior Parietal Cortex Interferes with Arm Trajectory Adjustments during the Learning of New Dynamics , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[44] Hiroshi Imamizu,et al. Human cerebellar activity reflecting an acquired internal model of a new tool , 2000, Nature.
[45] Amy J Bastian,et al. Cerebellar damage impairs automaticity of a recently practiced movement. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[46] R. Shadmehr,et al. Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.
[47] J. Gordon,et al. Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[48] Timothy D. Lee,et al. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .
[49] Axel Buchner,et al. Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task , 1998 .
[50] Konrad Paul Kording,et al. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.
[51] M. Ghilardi,et al. Patterns of regional brain activation associated with different forms of motor learning , 2000, Brain Research.
[52] R. Shadmehr,et al. A Gain-Field Encoding of Limb Position and Velocity in the Internal Model of Arm Dynamics , 2003, PLoS biology.
[53] E Bizzi,et al. Motor learning by field approximation. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[54] Rachael D. Seidler,et al. Differential effects of age on sequence learning and sensorimotor adaptation , 2006, Brain Research Bulletin.
[55] E. Todorov. Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[56] R A Scheidt,et al. Learning to move amid uncertainty. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[57] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[58] Kurt A. Thoroughman,et al. Rapid Reshaping of Human Motor Generalization , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[59] M. Nissen,et al. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[60] Yasmin L. Hashambhoy,et al. Neural Correlates of Reach Errors , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[61] Alexandre Pouget,et al. Computational approaches to sensorimotor transformations , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[62] David C. Knill,et al. Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control both the direction and distance of pointing movements , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[63] J. C. Johnston,et al. Attention and performance. , 2001, Annual review of psychology.
[64] R. Miall,et al. The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[65] M. A. Stadler,et al. Role of attention in implicit learning. , 1995 .
[66] Peter A. Frensch,et al. One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. , 1998 .
[67] Konrad Paul Körding,et al. The loss function of sensorimotor learning. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[68] K. Doya,et al. Parallel neural networks for learning sequential procedures , 1999, Trends in Neurosciences.
[69] P. Matthews,et al. Changing brain networks for visuomotor control with increased movement automaticity. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.
[70] F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al. Central representation of time during motor learning. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[71] P. Kuhl. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.