Self-initiation and temporal cueing of monaural tones reduce the auditory N1 and P2

Event-related potentials (ERPs) to tones that are self-initiated are reduced in their magnitude in comparison with ERPs to tones that are externally generated. This phenomenon has been taken as evidence for an efference copy of the motor command acting to suppress the sensory response. However, self-initiation provides a strong temporal cue for the stimulus which might also contribute to the ERP suppression for self-initiated tones. The current experiment sought to investigate the suppression of monaural tones by temporal cueing and also whether the addition of self-initiation enhanced this suppression. Lastly, the experiment sought to investigate the lateralisation of the ERP suppression via presenting these monaural tones to each ear respectively. We examined source waveforms extracted from the lateralised auditory cortices and measured the modulation of the N1 and P2 components by cueing and self-initiation. Self-initiation significantly reduced the amplitude of the N1 component. Temporal cueing without self-initiation significantly reduced the P2 component. There were no significant differences in the amplitude of either the N1 or the P2 between self-initiation and temporal cuing. There was a significant lateralisation effect on the N1—it being significantly larger contralateral to the ear of stimulation. There was no interaction between lateralisation and side of the temporal cue or side of self-initiation suggesting that the effects of self-initiation and temporal cuing are equal bilaterally. We conclude that a significant proportion of ERP suppression by self-initiation is a result of inherent temporal cueing.

[1]  T. Picton,et al.  The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the component structure. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[2]  Sonja A. Kotz,et al.  The Cerebellum Generates Motor-to-Auditory Predictions: ERP Lesion Evidence , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[3]  K. Lange The reduced N1 to self-generated tones: an effect of temporal predictability? , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[4]  J A MARAIST,et al.  Effects of auditory masking upon the speech of stutterers. , 1957, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.

[5]  P. Sowman,et al.  Threshold for Detection of Incisal Forces Is Increased by Jaw Movement , 2010, Journal of dental research.

[6]  T Allison,et al.  The scalp topography of human somatosensory and auditory evoked potentials. , 1977, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  E. Schröger,et al.  Suppression of the auditory N1 event-related potential component with unpredictable self-initiated tones: evidence for internal forward models with dynamic stimulation. , 2008, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[8]  Claudio Babiloni,et al.  “Gating” of human short-latency somatosensory evoked cortical responses during execution of movement. A high resolution electroencephalography study , 1999, Brain Research.

[9]  K. Reinikainen,et al.  Interstimulus interval and the selective-attention effect on auditory ERPs: "N1 enhancement" versus processing negativity. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[10]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[11]  Robert J. Barry,et al.  The auditory-evoked N2 and P3 components in the stop-signal task: Indices of inhibition, response-conflict or error-detection? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[12]  E. Schröger,et al.  Attenuated human auditory middle latency response and evoked 40‐Hz response to self‐initiated sounds , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  K. Lange Brain correlates of early auditory processing are attenuated by expectations for time and pitch , 2009, Brain and Cognition.

[14]  J. Connolly,et al.  Stability of pathway-hemisphere differences in the auditory event-related potential (ERP) to monaural stimulation. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[15]  R. Hari,et al.  Suppressed responses to self-triggered sounds in the human auditory cortex. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  Pamela Baess,et al.  Action–Sound Coincidences Suppress Evoked Responses of the Human Auditory Cortex in EEG and MEG , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  J. Ford,et al.  Neurophysiological evidence of corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.

[18]  J. Duysens,et al.  Gating of sensation and evoked potentials following foot stimulation during human gait , 1990, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  G. Curio,et al.  Speaking modifies voice‐evoked activity in the human auditory cortex , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[20]  E Donchin,et al.  The effects of temporal and event uncertainty in determining the waveforms of the auditory event related potential (ERP). , 1976, Psychophysiology.

[21]  J. Ford,et al.  Out-of-Synch and Out-of-Sorts: Dysfunction of Motor-Sensory Communication in Schizophrenia , 2008, Biological Psychiatry.

[22]  E. Schafer,et al.  Self-stimulation alters human sensory brain responses. , 1974, Science.

[23]  A. Amochaev,et al.  Knowledge of stimulus timing attenuates human evoked cortical potentials. , 1981, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[24]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Modulation of the Auditory Cortex during Speech: An MEG Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[25]  Srikantan S. Nagarajan,et al.  Motor-induced Suppression of the Auditory Cortex , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  J. Eggermont,et al.  Maturation of human central auditory system activity: separating auditory evoked potentials by dipole source modeling , 2002, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[27]  Judith M Ford,et al.  Dissecting corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[28]  P. Thier,et al.  Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one's actions. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[29]  Josef P. Rauschecker,et al.  An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor control and integration , 2011, Hearing Research.

[30]  R. Gur Motoric laterality imbalance in schizophrenia. A possible concomitant of left hemisphere dysfunction. , 1977, Archives of general psychiatry.

[31]  S. K. Barber,et al.  Knowledge of Stimulus Repetition Affects the Magnitude and Spatial Distribution of Low-Frequency Event-Related Brain Potentials , 2002, Audiology and Neurotology.

[32]  Carmen Weiss,et al.  The self in action effects: Selective attenuation of self-generated sounds , 2011, Cognition.

[33]  K M Heilman,et al.  Atypical cerebral laterality in adults with persistent developmental stuttering , 2003, Neurology.

[34]  Mingzhou Ding,et al.  Event-Related Potentials , 2006 .

[35]  Heidi E Kirsch,et al.  Single-Trial Speech Suppression of Auditory Cortex Activity in Humans , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  J. Ford,et al.  Fine-tuning of auditory cortex during speech production. , 2005, Psychophysiology.