Learning from Examples: Instructional Principles from the Worked Examples Research

Worked examples are instructional devices that provide an expert's problem solution for a learner to study. Worked-examples research is a cognitive-experimental program that has relevance to classroom instruction and the broader educational research community. A frame- work for organizing the findings of this research is proposed, leading to instructional design principles. For instance, one instructional design principle suggests that effective examples have highly integrated components. They employ multiple modalities in presentation and emphasize conceptual structure by labeling or segmenting. At the lesson level, effective instruction employs multiple examples for each conceptual problem type, varies example formats within problem type, and employs surface features to signal deep structure. Also, examples should be presented in close proximity to matched practice problems. More- over, learners can be encouraged through direct training or by the structure of the worked example to actively self:explain examples. Worked examples are associated with early stages of skill development, but the design principles are relevant to constructivist research and teaching.

[1]  William M. Smith,et al.  A Study of Thinking , 1956 .

[2]  L. E. Bourne,et al.  CONCEPT LEARNING AS A FUNCTION OF AVAILABILITY OF PREVIOUSLY PRESENTED INFORMATION. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.

[3]  Yves Nolet Ludwig Wittgenstein, Philosophische Untersuchungen. Philosophical Investigations. Translated by G. E. M. Anscombe , 1968 .

[4]  M. David Merrill,et al.  Exemplar and nonexampler variables which produce correct concept classification behavior and specified classification errors. , 1972 .

[5]  R. Glaser Components of a Psychology of Instruction: Toward a Science of Design , 1976 .

[6]  Peter H. Lindsay 10 – The representation of knowledge , 1977 .

[7]  Richard C. Anderson,et al.  Schooling and the Acquisition of Knowledge , 1978 .

[8]  “Schooling and the acquisition of knowledge” , 1979 .

[9]  Edward A. Silver,et al.  STUDENT PERCEPTIONS OF RELATEDNESS AMONG MATHEMATICAL VERBAL PROBLEMS , 1979 .

[10]  Paul J. Feltovich,et al.  Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices , 1981, Cogn. Sci..

[11]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Expertise in Problem Solving. , 1981 .

[12]  J. Sweller,et al.  Consequences of history-cued and means–end strategies in problem solving. , 1982 .

[13]  M. Levine,et al.  Effects of Goal Specificity on Means-Ends Analysis and Learning. , 1982 .

[14]  J. Sweller,et al.  Effects of Subgoal Density and Location on Learning during Problem Solving. , 1982 .

[15]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Schema induction and analogical transfer , 1983, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  John Sweller,et al.  Development of expertise in mathematical problem solving , 1983 .

[17]  S. Scribner Studying working intelligence. , 1984 .

[18]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Reciprocal Teaching of Comprehension-Fostering and Comprehension-Monitoring Activities , 1984 .

[19]  J. Sweller,et al.  What do students learn while solving mathematics problems , 1985 .

[20]  J. Sweller,et al.  The Use of Worked Examples as a Substitute for Problem Solving in Learning Algebra , 1985 .

[21]  Robert D. Tennyson,et al.  An Empirically Based Instructional Design Theory for Teaching Concepts , 1986 .

[22]  H. Simon,et al.  Learning Mathematics From Examples and by Doing , 1987 .

[23]  J. Sweller,et al.  Effects of schema acquisition and rule automation on mathematical problem-solving transfer. , 1987 .

[24]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Self-Explanations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning To Solve Problems. Technical Report No. 9. , 1987 .

[25]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Learning from Examples via Self-Explanations. Technical Report No. 11. , 1988 .

[26]  Michelene T. H. Chi,et al.  Learning from Examples Via Self-Explanations , 1988, Knowing, Learning, and Instruction.

[27]  Rand J. Spiro,et al.  Multiple analogies for complex concepts: antidotes for analogy-induced misconception in advanced knowledge acquisition , 1988 .

[28]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  THE MIND'S EYE IN CHESS , 1988 .

[29]  R. A. Tarmizi,et al.  Guidance during Mathematical Problem Solving. , 1988 .

[30]  K. VanLehn Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition , 1989 .

[31]  Brian H. Ross,et al.  Remindings in learning and instruction , 1989 .

[32]  L. Resnick,et al.  Knowing, Learning, and Instruction , 2018 .

[33]  Matthew W. Lewis,et al.  Self-Explonations: How Students Study and Use Examples in Learning to Solve Problems , 1989, Cogn. Sci..

[34]  K. Holyoak,et al.  Learning subgoals and methods for solving probability problems , 1990, Memory & cognition.

[35]  J. Sweller,et al.  Structuring Effective Worked Examples , 1990 .

[36]  N. Webb Task-Related Verbal Interaction and Mathematics Learning in Small Groups. , 1991 .

[37]  M. Chi,et al.  The Content of Physics Self-Explanations , 1991 .

[38]  Ok-choon Park,et al.  Hypermedia: functional features and research issues , 1991 .

[39]  Susan M. Williams,et al.  Putting Case-Based Instruction Into Context: Examples From Legal and Medical Education , 1992 .

[40]  F. Paas Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. , 1992 .

[41]  John R. Anderson Development of expertise , 1993 .

[42]  H. Van Engen AT Classic: Twentieth Century Mathematics for the Elementary School. , 1993 .

[43]  Randolph M. Jones,et al.  What Mediates the Self-explanation Eeect? Knowledge Gaps, Schemas or Analogies? , 1993 .

[44]  J. Bruer Schools for Thought: A Science of Learning in the Classroom , 1993 .

[45]  Kurt VanLehn,et al.  Better Learners Use Analogical Problem Solving Sparingly , 1993, ICML.

[46]  F. Paas,et al.  Variability of Worked Examples and Transfer of Geometrical Problem-Solving Skills: A Cognitive-Load Approach , 1994 .

[47]  K. McGilly,et al.  Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice. , 1994 .

[48]  Mitchell J. Nathan Learning through Self-Explanation of Mathematics Examples: Effects of Cognitive Load. , 1994 .

[49]  M. Chi,et al.  Eliciting Self‐Explanations Improves Understanding , 1994 .

[50]  R. Catrambone Improving examples to improve transfer to novel problems , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[51]  William M. Carroll Using worked examples as an instructional support in the algebra classroom. , 1994 .

[52]  Margaret M. Recker,et al.  Learning Strategies and Transfer in the Domain of Programming , 1994 .

[53]  Richard Catrambone,et al.  Aiding subgoal learning: Effects on transfer. , 1995 .

[54]  Ann L. Brown,et al.  Training in Self-Explanation and Self-Regulation Strategies: Investigating the Effects of Knowledge Acquisition Activities on Problem Solving , 1995 .

[55]  J. Sweller,et al.  Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes , 1995 .

[56]  Sharon Derry Psychological Foundations of the TiPS System: A Handbook for System 1.0. , 1995 .

[57]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Learning for later teaching: An exploration of mediational links between teaching expectancy and learning results. , 1995 .

[58]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  A schema-based theory of transfer. , 1996 .

[59]  R. Catrambone Generalizing Solution Procedures Learned From Examples , 1996 .

[60]  Wim H. Gijselaers,et al.  Bringing Problem-Based Learning To Higher Education: Theory And Practice , 1996 .

[61]  S. Stein What's All the Fuss About? , 1996 .

[62]  The role of educational psychology in the preparation of teachers , 1996 .

[63]  Jill L. Quilici,et al.  Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. , 1996 .

[64]  Alexander Renkl,et al.  Learning from Worked-Out-Examples: A Study on Individual Differences , 1997, Cogn. Sci..

[65]  Learning by Explaining--Or Better by Listening?. , 1997 .

[66]  P. Chandler,et al.  The Role of Visual Indicators in Dual Sensory Mode Instruction , 1997 .

[67]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The role of examples and rules in the acquisition of a cognitive skill. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[68]  Alexander Renkl Lernen durch Lehren , 1997 .

[69]  Alexander Renkl Lernen durch Lehren : zentrale Wirkmechanismen beim kooperativen Lernen , 1997 .

[70]  R. Mayer,et al.  Multimedia learning: Are we asking the right questions? , 1997 .

[71]  Yair Neuman,et al.  Is self-explanation while solving problems helpful? The case of analogical problem-solving , 1998 .

[72]  R. Catrambone The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples so that students can solve novel problems. , 1998 .

[73]  H. Mandl,et al.  Learning from Worked-Out Examples: The Effects of Example Variability and Elicited Self-Explanations , 1998, Contemporary educational psychology.

[74]  J. Sweller,et al.  Learning to Solve Compare Word Problems: The Effect of Example Format and Generating Self-Explanations , 1998 .

[75]  F. Paas,et al.  Cognitive Architecture and Instructional Design , 1998 .

[76]  R. Mayer,et al.  Maximizing Constructivist Learning From Multimedia Communications by Minimizing Cognitive Load , 1999 .

[77]  Sharon J. Derry,et al.  Fostering Students' Statistical and Scientific Thinking: Lessons Learned From an Innovative College Course , 2000 .

[78]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Use of examples and procedures in problem solving , 1991 .

[79]  Richard K. Staley,et al.  From Example Study to Problem Solving: Smooth Transitions Help Learning , 2002 .