Social Competence in Infancy: A Model of Parent-Infant Interaction.

us out of the "dark ages" during which we believed that the infant began life as a passive blob confronted with the task of making sense out of sensory chaos. Our current view considers the newborn infant to be active, capable of organizing complex information, selectively attentive, and capable of rapid learning. Upon careful examination, it appears that our acceptance of the young infant as a competent though immature organism has been largely confined to the domain of per ceptual and cognitive skills. When we consider research on early social relations, we find that while we were documenting the "remark able" perceptual and cognitive abilities of young infants, early social development continued to be studied under the "passive blob model" of the infant. Recognition of the infant as an active and competent contributor to the developing social relationships with caretakers has been late in coming, and in spite of several recent contributions (e.g., Lewis & Rosenblum, 1974) is not yet widespread. The purpose of this paper is to make explicit the ways in which the competent infant is instrumental in establishing those social con ditions which are considered supportive of infant development. I will argue that the infant is preadapted to be selectively attentive to the kinds of stimulation provided by people and that the infant is equipped with a repertoire of behaviors which effectively capture adult attention and facilitate effective adult-infant interactions. This, in turn, facilitates development. Moreover, I wish to argue that the central reciprocity in early social relationships between infants and caretakers is mediated by mutual enhancement of "feelings of efficacy" as defined by White (1959). The model which I propose focuses upon conditions that con tribute to feelings of efficacy generated in caretakers and infants by their interactions, namely the extent to which each member of the dyad provides the other with contingency experience. Although I mean

[1]  E. F. Boyd Visual fixation and voice discrimination in 2-month-old infants. , 1974, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[2]  P. Leiderman,et al.  The effects of denial of early mother-infant interaction on maternal self-confidence. , 1973, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[3]  D. Pederson,et al.  The influence of amplitude and frequency of vestibular stimulation on the activity of two-month-old infants. , 1973, Child development.

[4]  D R Pederson,et al.  The effects of vertical rocking frequencies on the arousal level in two-month-old infants. , 1973, Child development.

[5]  M. Ainsworth,et al.  Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. , 1972, Child development.

[6]  J. Williams,et al.  Effects of mother-infant separation on maternal attachment behavior. , 1972, Child development.

[7]  J. Watson Smiling, cooing, and "the game." , 1972 .

[8]  E. Thoman,et al.  The relative efficacy of contact and vestibular-proprioceptive stimulation in soothing neonates. , 1972, Child development.

[9]  S. Fraiberg Intervention in infancy: a program for blind infants. , 1971, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.

[10]  J. A. Ambrose,et al.  Stimulation in Early Infancy , 1970 .

[11]  H. Moss,et al.  Patterns and determinants of maternal attachment. , 1970, The Journal of pediatrics.

[12]  M. Lewis,et al.  PERCEPTUAL‐COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT IN INFANCY: A GENERALIZED EXPECTANCY MODEL AS A FUNCTION OF THE MOTHER‐INFANT INTERACTION1 , 1968 .

[13]  H. Birch,et al.  Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children , 1968 .

[14]  R. A. Haaf,et al.  A facial dimension in visual discrimination by human infants. , 1967, Child development.

[15]  A. Korner,et al.  VISUAL ALERTNESS AS RELATED TO SOOTHING IN NEONATES: IMPLICATIONS FOR MATERNAL STIMULATION AND EARLY DEPRIVATION. , 1966 .

[16]  C Ounsted,et al.  The biological significance of gaze aversion with particular reference to the syndrome of infantile autism. , 1966, Behavioral science.

[17]  B. Birns,et al.  The Effectiveness of Various Soothing Techniques on Human Neonates , 1966, Psychosomatic medicine.

[18]  M. Haith,et al.  The response of the human newborn to visual movement. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[19]  P. Salapatek,et al.  Visual scanning of triangles by the human newborn. , 1966, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[20]  H. Blauvelt Capacity of a human neonate reflex to signal future response by present action. , 1962, Child development.

[21]  R. W. White Motivation reconsidered: the concept of competence. , 1959, Psychological review.

[22]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[23]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  chapter 6 – Speech Perception in Early Infancy1 , 1975 .

[24]  C. Ramey Nutrition, Response-Contingent Stimulation, and the Maternal Deprivation Syndrome: Results of an Early Intervention Program. , 1975 .

[25]  Leonard A. Rosenblum,et al.  The Effect of the Infant on Its Caregiver. Volume 1 in the Origins of Behavior Series. , 1974 .

[26]  G. C. Carpenter,et al.  Visual Regard of Moving and Stationary Faces in Early Infancy. , 1974 .

[27]  Daniel N. Stern,et al.  Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviors. , 1974 .

[28]  S. Trehub,et al.  Auditory-Linguistic Sensitivity in Early Infancy , 1972 .

[29]  M. Lewis,et al.  Infant development in lower-class American families. , 1972, Human development.

[30]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Origins of Intelligence in Children , 1971 .

[31]  S. Fraiberg Parallel and divergent patterns in blind and sighted infants. , 1968, The Psychoanalytic study of the child.

[32]  J. Watson Memory and "contingency analysis" in infant learning. , 1967 .

[33]  H. Moss Sex, Age, and State as Determinants of Mother-Infant Interaction* , 1967 .

[34]  P. Wolff The causes, controls, and organization of behavior in the neonate. , 1966, Psychological issues.

[35]  J. Hunt,et al.  Intelligence and experience , 1961 .