Subcortical locus of temporal coupling in the bimanual movements of a callosotomy patient

The timing of repetitive movements was assessed in a callosotomy patient under unimanual and bimanual conditions. Similar to neurologically healthy individuals, the patient exhibited strong temporal coupling in the bimanual condition. Moreover, for both the left and right hands, within-hand temporal variability was reduced in the bimanual condition compared to the unimanual conditions. This bimanual advantage is hypothesized to reflect the temporal integration of separable timing signals, one associated with the left hand and one associated with the right hand (Helmuth, L. L., & Ivry, R. B. (1996). When two hands are better than one: Reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 278‐293). The fact that it persists following callosotomy is inconsistent with models that attribute bimanual coordination in these patients to the control of a single hemisphere. Rather, the results suggest that motor commands from the two hemispheres are integrated subcortically. ” 1999 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

[1]  A. P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Functional magnetic resonance imaging of motor cortex: hemispheric asymmetry and handedness. , 1993, Science.

[2]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Interhemispheric integration: II. Symmetry and convergence of the corticostriatal projections of the left and the right principal sulcus (PS) and the left and the right supplementary motor area (SMA) of the rhesus monkey. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  M. Gazzaniga,et al.  Dissociation of Spatial and Temporal Coupling in the Bimanual Movements of Callosotomy Patients , 1996 .

[4]  M. Peters,et al.  Coordination of the two hands and effects of attentional manipulation in the production of a bimanual 2:3 polyrhythm , 1989 .

[5]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Independent hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain patients , 1989, Nature.

[6]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: advances and challenges , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[7]  R. Ivry,et al.  When two hands are better than one: reduced timing variability during bimanual movements. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  A. Kristofferson,et al.  The timing of interresponse intervals , 1973 .

[9]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990 .

[10]  Richard lvry,et al.  Cerebellar timing systems. , 1997 .

[11]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Neural mechanisms of timing , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  S. Klein,et al.  Positional uncertainty in peripheral and amblyopic vision , 1987, Vision Research.

[13]  J. Deniau,et al.  Disinhibition as a basic process in the expression of striatal functions , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  W. Meck Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Enhanced dual task performance following corpus commissurotomy in humans , 1985, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Independent Attentional Scanning in the Separated Hemispheres of Split-Brain Patients , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation , 1988 .

[18]  R. Ivry Cerebellar timing systems. , 1997, International review of neurobiology.

[19]  D Goodman,et al.  On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Bimanual Movement Control: Information processing and Interaction Effects , 1984 .

[21]  R. Ivry The representation of temporal information in perception and motor control , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[22]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[23]  P Viviani,et al.  Cerebral dominance and asynchrony between bimanual two-dimensional movements. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  M. Turvey,et al.  Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in the visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  A. Opstal Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior , 1995 .

[26]  Michael S. Gazzaniga,et al.  Subcortical transfer of higher order information: More illusory than real? , 1995 .

[27]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Production of polyrhythms. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[28]  G. E. Stelmach,et al.  The Timing Effects of Accent Production in Periodic Finger-Tapping Sequences. , 1996, Journal of motor behavior.

[29]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation. , 1988, Psychological review.

[30]  J. Kelso Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. , 1984, The American journal of physiology.

[31]  P. Gloor,et al.  The connections of the amygdala and of the anterior temporal cortex in the human brain , 1960, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[32]  S. Grillner,et al.  Neuronal network generating locomotor behavior in lamprey: circuitry, transmitters, membrane properties, and simulation. , 1991, Annual review of neuroscience.

[33]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  On marching to two different drummers: perceptual aspects of the difficulties. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[34]  Richard J. Jagacinski,et al.  On marching to two different drummers: perceptual aspects of the difficulties. , 1985 .

[35]  M. Wiesendanger,et al.  Temporal Structure of a Bimanual Goal‐directed Movement Sequence in Monkeys , 1994, The European journal of neuroscience.

[36]  G. Ermentrout Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior , 1997 .

[37]  S. Keele,et al.  Timing Functions of The Cerebellum , 1989, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[38]  R. B. Ivry,et al.  Reduced Timing Variability in Patients with Unilateral Cerebellar Lesions during Bimanual Movements , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[39]  D. Heeger,et al.  Comparison of contrast-normalization and threshold models of the responses of simple cells in cat striate cortex , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[40]  A. Wing 28 The Long and Short of Timing in Response Sequences , 1980 .

[41]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Tests of parallel versus integrated structure in polyrhythmic tapping. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[42]  R. Ivry,et al.  Perception and production of temporal intervals across a range of durations: evidence for a common timing mechanism. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  P. N. Kugler,et al.  Fluctuations and phase symmetry in coordinated rhythmic movements. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[44]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  A Computational Model of How the Basal Ganglia Produce Sequences , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.