Expectancy, value and psychological distance: A new look at goal gradients

Goal gradients refer to the increase in motivation as a function of goal proximity. We propose that motivation does not always increase closer to the goal, and that in order to predict the shape and steepness of goal gradients one needs to look at how distance affects the two components of motivation—expectancy and value. Furthermore, we distinguish between four aspects of expectancy (probability, difficulty, sufficiency, necessity) and two types of value (value related to high level construal, value related to low level construal), each of which has a unique distance-related dynamics. It is proposed that motivational gradients are determined by the effect that distance has on each of these components. Our study demonstrated gradients of motivation, sufficiency and necessity, but not difficulty. We discuss whether avoidance gradients would be necessarily steeper than approach gradients, as is postulated by Miller’s (1944) conflict models. We also suggest that in some situations (e.g., when gradients reflect necessity) gradients would be moderated by regulatory focus (e.g., would be steeper in a prevention focus than in a promotion focus, Higgins, 1998).

[1]  W. Edwards The theory of decision making. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Thomas Gilovich,et al.  "What, Me Worry?": Arousal, Misattribution, and the Effect of Temporal Distance on Confidence , 1998 .

[3]  Jens Förster,et al.  Approach and avoidance strength during goal attainment : Regulatory focus and the goal looms larger effect , 1998 .

[4]  Howard Rachlin,et al.  Self-control: Beyond commitment. , 1995 .

[5]  S. Epstein,et al.  Relative steepness of approach and avoidance gradients as a function of magnitude and valence of incentive. , 1977 .

[6]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Decision making over time and under uncertainty: a common approach , 1991 .

[7]  A. Bandura Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy.

[8]  Ying Zhang,et al.  The dynamics of self-regulation , 2009 .

[9]  N. Feather Expectations and Actions , 2021 .

[10]  V. Vroom Work and motivation , 1964 .

[11]  W. Mischel Processes in Delay of Gratification , 1974 .

[12]  Lawrence A. Pervin,et al.  Goal concepts in personality and social psychology. , 1989 .

[13]  B. Skinner,et al.  Principles of Behavior , 1944 .

[14]  J. Elster,et al.  Utility from memory and anticipation. , 1992 .

[15]  N. Lewis A dynamic theory of personality , 1935 .

[16]  Approach and avoidance gradients and conflict behavior in a predominantly temporal situation. , 1954 .

[17]  Matthew Rabin,et al.  Addiction and Present-Biased Preferences , 2002 .

[18]  S. Sherman On the self-erasing nature of errors of prediction. , 1980 .

[19]  N. Smith An empirical determination of an approach gradient. , 1960, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[20]  I. Ajzen,et al.  Attitudes toward objects as predictors of sin-gle and multiple behavioral criteria , 1974 .

[21]  N. Liberman,et al.  Distinguishing Gains from Nonlosses and Losses from Nongains: A Regulatory Focus Perspective on Hedonic Intensity , 2000 .

[22]  Y. Trope,et al.  The pros and cons of temporally near and distant action. , 2004, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Time-dependent gambling: odds now, money later. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  M. Berkun,et al.  Displacement as a function of conflict. , 1955, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[25]  J. S. Brown,et al.  Gradients of approach and avoidance responses and their relation to level of motivation. , 1948, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[26]  Y. Trope,et al.  Seeing the forest when entry is unlikely: probability and the mental representation of events. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[27]  Y. Trope,et al.  Counteractive self-control in overcoming temptation. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[28]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Temporal construal theory of intertemporal judgment and decision , 2003 .

[29]  Thomas C. Schelling,et al.  Self-Command in Practice, in Policy, and in a Theory of Rational Choice , 1984 .

[30]  Daniel W. Russell,et al.  The cognition–emotion process in achievement-related contexts. , 1979 .

[31]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Mixing virtue and vice: combining the immediacy effect and the diversification heuristic , 1999 .

[32]  N Liberman,et al.  Promotion and prevention focus on alternative hypotheses: implications for attributional functions. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  Albert Bandura,et al.  Self-regulation of motivation and action through internal standards and goal systems. , 1989 .

[34]  N. Miller,et al.  Displacement: greater generalization of approach than avoidance in a generalized approach avoidance conflict. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  Lidija Dimkovska WHAT AND WHY , 2005, Causes and Consequences of Global Migration.

[36]  Koch Sigmund Ed,et al.  Psychology: A Study of A Science , 1962 .

[37]  W. Edwards Probability-preferences among bets with differing expected values. , 1954, The American journal of psychology.

[38]  N. W. Smith GSR Measures of Cigarette Smokers’ Temporal Approach and Avoidance Gradients , 1965 .

[39]  Jens Förster,et al.  Success/Failure Feedback, Expectancies, and Approach/Avoidance Motivation: How Regulatory Focus Moderates Classic Relations , 2001 .

[40]  M. Ross,et al.  Egocentric Biases in Availability and Attribution , 1979 .

[41]  R. Baumeister,et al.  Self-Regulation Failure: An Overview , 1996 .

[42]  K. Lewin,et al.  Field Theory in Social Science: Selected Theoretical Papers , 1951 .

[43]  Y. Trope,et al.  The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. , 1998 .

[44]  T. Gilovich,et al.  Effect of Temporal Perspective on Subjective Confidence , 1993 .

[45]  E. Higgins Beyond pleasure and pain. , 1997, The American psychologist.

[46]  R. Hogarth,et al.  Judging probable cause. , 1986 .

[47]  G. Semin,et al.  Language use in intergroup contexts: the linguistic intergroup bias. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[48]  T. Gjesme Goal distance in time and its effects on the relations between achievement motives and performance , 1974 .

[49]  John W. Atkinson,et al.  Introduction: Motivation , 1964 .

[50]  E. Higgins,et al.  Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. , 1996 .

[51]  R. Thaler,et al.  An Economic Theory of Self-Control , 1977, Journal of Political Economy.

[52]  Yaacov Trope,et al.  Transcending the "here": the effect of spatial distance on social judgment. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[53]  J. Hunt,et al.  Personality and the behavior disorders , 1944 .

[54]  Y. Trope,et al.  Temporal construal and time-dependent changes in preference. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[55]  J. Elster Ulysses and the Sirens: Studies in Rationality and Irrationality , 1985 .

[56]  E. Deci,et al.  The "What" and "Why" of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior , 2000 .

[57]  G. Ainslie Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[58]  G. Loewenstein,et al.  Time Discounting and Time Preference: A Critical Review , 2002 .

[59]  N. Miller,et al.  Displacement and conflict; learnable drive as a basis for the steeper gradient of avoidance than of approach. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[60]  J. Rotter Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. , 1966, Psychological monographs.

[61]  Peter H. M. P. Roelofsma,et al.  Immediacy and Certainty in Intertemporal Choice , 1995 .