Anticipation and Future-Oriented Capabilities in Natural and Artificial Cognition

Empirical evidence indicates that anticipatory representations grounded in the sensorimotor neural apparatus are crucially involved in several low and high level cognitive functions, including attention, motor control, planning, and goal-oriented behavior. A unitary theoretical framework is emerging that emphasizes how simulative capabilities enable social abilities, too, including joint attention, imitation, perspective taking and communication. We argue that anticipation will be a key element for bootstrapping high level cognitive functions in cognitive robotics, too. We thus propose the challenge of understanding how anticipatory representations, that serve for coordinating with the future and not only with the present, develop in situated agents.

[1]  Hod Lipson,et al.  Resilient Machines Through Continuous Self-Modeling , 2006, Science.

[2]  A. Borst Seeing smells: imaging olfactory learning in bees , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  Stevan Harnad The Symbol Grounding Problem , 1999, ArXiv.

[4]  Olivier Sigaud,et al.  Anticipatory Behavior in Adaptive Learning Systems: Foundations, Theories, and Systems , 2003 .

[5]  The Inner Sense of Action , 2000 .

[6]  L. Steels Intelligence — Dynamics and Representations , 1995 .

[7]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[8]  N. Wiener,et al.  Behavior, Purpose and Teleology , 1943, Philosophy of Science.

[9]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[10]  M. Arbib,et al.  Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  Cristiano Castelfranchi,et al.  Guarantees for Autonomy in Cognitive Agent Architecture , 1995, ECAI Workshop on Agent Theories, Architectures, and Languages.

[12]  Nuttapong Chentanez,et al.  Intrinsically Motivated Reinforcement Learning , 2004, NIPS.

[13]  Tom Ziemke,et al.  Internal simulation of perception: a minimal neuro-robotic model , 2005, Neurocomputing.

[14]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[15]  Peter Gärdenfors,et al.  The evolution of anticipatory cognition as a precursor to symbolic communication , 2005 .

[16]  Marco C. Bettoni,et al.  Made-Up Minds: A Constructivist Approach to Artificial Intelligence , 1993, IEEE Expert.

[17]  K. Doya Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  Deb Roy,et al.  Semiotic schemas: A framework for grounding language in action and perception , 2005, Artif. Intell..

[19]  M. Iacoboni Understanding Others: Imitation, Language, Empathy , 2002 .

[20]  Kenji Doya,et al.  Metalearning and neuromodulation , 2002, Neural Networks.

[21]  F. Keijzer Representation and Behavior , 2001 .

[22]  Randall D. Beer,et al.  The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  Alexander Gloye,et al.  FU-Fighters Small Size 2004 , 2004 .

[24]  Stefano Nolfi,et al.  Behaviour as a Complex Adaptive System: On the Role of Self-Organization in the Development of Individual and Collective Behaviour , 2004, Complexus.

[25]  Christian Balkenius,et al.  Attention as selection-for-action: a scheme for active perception , 1999, 1999 Third European Workshop on Advanced Mobile Robots (Eurobot'99). Proceedings (Cat. No.99EX355).

[26]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  A. Goldman,et al.  Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[28]  Cristiano Castelfranchi Mind as an Anticipatory Device: For a Theory of Expectations , 2005, BVAI.

[29]  R. Miall,et al.  Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. , 2003, Neuroreport.

[30]  F. W. Irwin Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men , 1932, The Psychological Clinic.

[31]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[32]  A. Clark Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again , 1996 .

[33]  Deb Roy,et al.  RIPLEY , HAND ME THE CUP ! ( SENSORIMOTOR REPRESENTATIONS FOR GROUNDING WORD MEANING ) , 2003 .

[34]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[35]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[36]  Aude Billard,et al.  Action Selection and Mental Transformation Based on a Chain of Forward Models , 2004 .

[37]  Martin V. Butz,et al.  An Analysis of the Ideomotor Principle and TOTE , 2006 .

[38]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[39]  D. Wolpert,et al.  The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.

[40]  Andy Clark,et al.  Towards a Cognitive Robotics , 1999, Adapt. Behav..

[41]  H. L. Roitblat,et al.  Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory , 1980 .

[42]  Lambert Schomaker,et al.  Anticipation in cybernetic systems: a case against mindless anti-representationalism , 2004, 2004 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics (IEEE Cat. No.04CH37583).

[43]  R. Schubotz Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  R. A. Brooks,et al.  Intelligence without Representation , 1991, Artif. Intell..

[46]  P. Strick,et al.  Basal Ganglia Output and Cognition: Evidence from Anatomical, Behavioral, and Clinical Studies , 2000, Brain and Cognition.

[47]  Giovanni Pezzulo,et al.  A Schema Based Model of the Praying Mantis , 2006, SAB.

[48]  Thomas Metzinger,et al.  Motor ontology: the representational reality of goals, actions and selves , 2003 .

[49]  M. Jeannerod Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.

[50]  M. Graziano,et al.  Effects of auditory and visual interference on auditory-visual delayed matching to sample in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[51]  A. Noë,et al.  A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[52]  Mark H. Bickhard,et al.  Foundational issues in artificial intelligence and cognitive science - impasse and solution , 1995, Advances in psychology.

[53]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Predictive Representations of State , 2001, NIPS.

[54]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Common codes for situated interaction , 2001 .

[55]  K. J. Craik,et al.  The nature of explanation , 1944 .

[56]  P. Redgrave,et al.  The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.

[57]  Yiannis Demiris,et al.  Hierarchical attentive multiple models for execution and recognition of actions , 2006, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[58]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[59]  Jason Fleischer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Anticipation in Cerebellum, Basal Ganglia, and Hippocampus , 2007, SAB ABiALS.

[60]  J. Adams,et al.  A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.

[61]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Anticipated Action Effects Affect the Selection, Initiation, and Execution of Actions , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[62]  Norbert Krüger,et al.  Three Dilemmas of Signal- and Symbol-Based Representations in Computer Vision , 2005, BVAI.

[63]  Cristiano Castelfranchi,et al.  The symbol detachment problem , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[64]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[65]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[66]  Stefano Nolfi,et al.  Learning to perceive the world as articulated: an approach for hierarchical learning in sensory-motor systems , 1998, Neural Networks.

[67]  Rolf Pfeifer,et al.  Understanding intelligence , 2020, Inequality by Design.

[68]  Stewart W. Wilson,et al.  From Animals to Animats 5. Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior , 1997 .

[69]  Joachim Hoffmann,et al.  Anticipatory Behavioral Control , 2003, ABiALS.

[70]  Luc Steels The biology and technology of intelligent autonomous agents , 1995, Robotics Auton. Syst..

[71]  Bartlett W. Mel Vision-based robot motion planning , 1990 .

[72]  G. Fricchione Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .

[73]  Martin V. Butz,et al.  Anticipatory Behavior in Adaptive Learning Systems , 2003, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.

[74]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[75]  Jean Decety,et al.  When the self represents the other: A new cognitive neuroscience view on psychological identification , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.

[76]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[77]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[78]  C. Campbell,et al.  On Being There , 1965 .

[79]  G. Hesslow Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[80]  Horst-Michael Groß,et al.  Visuomotor Anticipation - a Powerful Approach to Behavior-Driven Perception , 2003, Künstliche Intell..

[81]  Martin V. Butz,et al.  Internal Models and Anticipations in Adaptive Learning Systems , 2003, ABiALS.

[82]  Jun Tani,et al.  Learning to generate articulated behavior through the bottom-up and the top-down interaction processes , 2003, Neural Networks.

[83]  John Hallam,et al.  From Animals to Animats 10 , 2008 .

[84]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[85]  A. Damasio Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York (Grosset/Putnam) 1994. , 1994 .

[86]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Forward models in visuomotor control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[87]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[88]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[89]  J. Decety Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.