Anticipation and Future-Oriented Capabilities in Natural and Artificial Cognition
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Hod Lipson,et al. Resilient Machines Through Continuous Self-Modeling , 2006, Science.
[2] A. Borst. Seeing smells: imaging olfactory learning in bees , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[3] Stevan Harnad. The Symbol Grounding Problem , 1999, ArXiv.
[4] Olivier Sigaud,et al. Anticipatory Behavior in Adaptive Learning Systems: Foundations, Theories, and Systems , 2003 .
[5] The Inner Sense of Action , 2000 .
[6] L. Steels. Intelligence — Dynamics and Representations , 1995 .
[7] E. Reed. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .
[8] N. Wiener,et al. Behavior, Purpose and Teleology , 1943, Philosophy of Science.
[9] L. Barsalou,et al. Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[10] M. Arbib,et al. Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[11] Cristiano Castelfranchi,et al. Guarantees for Autonomy in Cognitive Agent Architecture , 1995, ECAI Workshop on Agent Theories, Architectures, and Languages.
[12] Nuttapong Chentanez,et al. Intrinsically Motivated Reinforcement Learning , 2004, NIPS.
[13] Tom Ziemke,et al. Internal simulation of perception: a minimal neuro-robotic model , 2005, Neurocomputing.
[14] Mitsuo Kawato,et al. Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[15] Peter Gärdenfors,et al. The evolution of anticipatory cognition as a precursor to symbolic communication , 2005 .
[16] Marco C. Bettoni,et al. Made-Up Minds: A Constructivist Approach to Artificial Intelligence , 1993, IEEE Expert.
[17] K. Doya. Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum in learning and motor control , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[18] Deb Roy,et al. Semiotic schemas: A framework for grounding language in action and perception , 2005, Artif. Intell..
[19] M. Iacoboni. Understanding Others: Imitation, Language, Empathy , 2002 .
[20] Kenji Doya,et al. Metalearning and neuromodulation , 2002, Neural Networks.
[21] F. Keijzer. Representation and Behavior , 2001 .
[22] Randall D. Beer,et al. The brain has a body: adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body and environment , 1997, Trends in Neurosciences.
[23] Alexander Gloye,et al. FU-Fighters Small Size 2004 , 2004 .
[24] Stefano Nolfi,et al. Behaviour as a Complex Adaptive System: On the Role of Self-Organization in the Development of Individual and Collective Behaviour , 2004, Complexus.
[25] Christian Balkenius,et al. Attention as selection-for-action: a scheme for active perception , 1999, 1999 Third European Workshop on Advanced Mobile Robots (Eurobot'99). Proceedings (Cat. No.99EX355).
[26] Scott T. Grafton,et al. Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[27] A. Goldman,et al. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[28] Cristiano Castelfranchi. Mind as an Anticipatory Device: For a Theory of Expectations , 2005, BVAI.
[29] R. Miall,et al. Connecting mirror neurons and forward models. , 2003, Neuroreport.
[30] F. W. Irwin. Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men , 1932, The Psychological Clinic.
[31] J. Piaget. The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .
[32] A. Clark. Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again , 1996 .
[33] Deb Roy,et al. RIPLEY , HAND ME THE CUP ! ( SENSORIMOTOR REPRESENTATIONS FOR GROUNDING WORD MEANING ) , 2003 .
[34] D. Spalding. The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.
[35] Rick Grush,et al. The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[36] Aude Billard,et al. Action Selection and Mental Transformation Based on a Chain of Forward Models , 2004 .
[37] Martin V. Butz,et al. An Analysis of the Ideomotor Principle and TOTE , 2006 .
[38] Daniel M. Wolpert,et al. Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.
[39] D. Wolpert,et al. The cerebellum is involved in predicting the sensory consequences of action , 1999, Neuroreport.
[40] Andy Clark,et al. Towards a Cognitive Robotics , 1999, Adapt. Behav..
[41] H. L. Roitblat,et al. Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory , 1980 .
[42] Lambert Schomaker,et al. Anticipation in cybernetic systems: a case against mindless anti-representationalism , 2004, 2004 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics (IEEE Cat. No.04CH37583).
[43] R. Schubotz. Prediction of external events with our motor system: towards a new framework , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] Linda B. Smith,et al. A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[45] R. A. Brooks,et al. Intelligence without Representation , 1991, Artif. Intell..
[46] P. Strick,et al. Basal Ganglia Output and Cognition: Evidence from Anatomical, Behavioral, and Clinical Studies , 2000, Brain and Cognition.
[47] Giovanni Pezzulo,et al. A Schema Based Model of the Praying Mantis , 2006, SAB.
[48] Thomas Metzinger,et al. Motor ontology: the representational reality of goals, actions and selves , 2003 .
[49] M. Jeannerod. Neural Simulation of Action: A Unifying Mechanism for Motor Cognition , 2001, NeuroImage.
[50] M. Graziano,et al. Effects of auditory and visual interference on auditory-visual delayed matching to sample in monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.
[51] A. Noë,et al. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[52] Mark H. Bickhard,et al. Foundational issues in artificial intelligence and cognitive science - impasse and solution , 1995, Advances in psychology.
[53] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Predictive Representations of State , 2001, NIPS.
[54] J. Kalaska,et al. Common codes for situated interaction , 2001 .
[55] K. J. Craik,et al. The nature of explanation , 1944 .
[56] P. Redgrave,et al. The basal ganglia: a vertebrate solution to the selection problem? , 1999, Neuroscience.
[57] Yiannis Demiris,et al. Hierarchical attentive multiple models for execution and recognition of actions , 2006, Robotics Auton. Syst..
[58] D. Wolpert,et al. Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. , 2000, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[59] Jason Fleischer,et al. Neural Correlates of Anticipation in Cerebellum, Basal Ganglia, and Hippocampus , 2007, SAB ABiALS.
[60] J. Adams,et al. A closed-loop theory of motor learning. , 1971, Journal of motor behavior.
[61] J. Hoffmann,et al. Anticipated Action Effects Affect the Selection, Initiation, and Execution of Actions , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[62] Norbert Krüger,et al. Three Dilemmas of Signal- and Symbol-Based Representations in Computer Vision , 2005, BVAI.
[63] Cristiano Castelfranchi,et al. The symbol detachment problem , 2007, Cognitive Processing.
[64] E. Holst,et al. Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.
[65] Peter Dayan,et al. A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.
[66] Stefano Nolfi,et al. Learning to perceive the world as articulated: an approach for hierarchical learning in sensory-motor systems , 1998, Neural Networks.
[67] Rolf Pfeifer,et al. Understanding intelligence , 2020, Inequality by Design.
[68] Stewart W. Wilson,et al. From Animals to Animats 5. Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior , 1997 .
[69] Joachim Hoffmann,et al. Anticipatory Behavioral Control , 2003, ABiALS.
[70] Luc Steels. The biology and technology of intelligent autonomous agents , 1995, Robotics Auton. Syst..
[71] Bartlett W. Mel. Vision-based robot motion planning , 1990 .
[72] G. Fricchione. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain , 1995 .
[73] Martin V. Butz,et al. Anticipatory Behavior in Adaptive Learning Systems , 2003, Lecture Notes in Computer Science.
[74] J. Decety,et al. From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.
[75] Jean Decety,et al. When the self represents the other: A new cognitive neuroscience view on psychological identification , 2003, Consciousness and Cognition.
[76] K. Doya,et al. A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[77] D M Wolpert,et al. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.
[78] C. Campbell,et al. On Being There , 1965 .
[79] G. Hesslow. Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[80] Horst-Michael Groß,et al. Visuomotor Anticipation - a Powerful Approach to Behavior-Driven Perception , 2003, Künstliche Intell..
[81] Martin V. Butz,et al. Internal Models and Anticipations in Adaptive Learning Systems , 2003, ABiALS.
[82] Jun Tani,et al. Learning to generate articulated behavior through the bottom-up and the top-down interaction processes , 2003, Neural Networks.
[83] John Hallam,et al. From Animals to Animats 10 , 2008 .
[84] Michael I. Jordan,et al. An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.
[85] A. Damasio. Descartes’ Error. Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York (Grosset/Putnam) 1994. , 1994 .
[86] Stefan Schaal,et al. Forward models in visuomotor control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.
[87] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[88] Michael I. Jordan,et al. Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..
[89] J. Decety. Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.