Cognitive Deficit and Poverty in the First 5 Years of Childhood in Bangladesh

OBJECTIVE: We aimed to determine the timing and size of the cognitive deficit associated with poverty in the first 5 years of life and to examine the role of parental characteristics, pre- and postnatal growth, and stimulation in the home in Bangladeshi children. We hypothesized that the effect of poverty on cognition begins in infancy and is mainly mediated by these factors. METHODS: We enrolled 2853 singletons, a subsample from a pregnancy supplementation trial in a poor rural area. We assessed mental development at 7, 18, and 64 months; anthropometry at birth, 12, 24, and 64 months; home stimulation at 18 and 64 months; and family’s socioeconomic background. In multiple regression analyses, we examined the effect of poverty at birth on IQ at 64 months and the extent that other factors mediated the effect. RESULTS: A mean cognitive deficit of 0.2 (95% confidence interval –0.4 to –0.02) z scores between the first and fifth wealth quintiles was apparent at 7 months and increased to 1.2 (95% confidence interval –1.3 to –1.0) z scores of IQ by 64 months. Parental education, pre- and postnatal growth in length, and home stimulation mediated 86% of the effects of poverty on IQ and had independent effects. Growth in the first 2 years had larger effects than later growth. Home stimulation had effects throughout the period. CONCLUSIONS: Effects of poverty on children’s cognition are mostly mediated through parental education, birth size, growth in the first 24 months, and home stimulation in the first 5 years.

[1]  Norbert R. Schady Parents' education, mothers' vocabulary, and cognitive development in early childhood: longitudinal evidence from Ecuador. , 2011, American journal of public health.

[2]  R. Martorell,et al.  Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain during early life with adult health and human capital in countries of low and middle income: findings from five birth cohort studies , 2013, The Lancet.

[3]  P. Willatts Development of means-end behavior in young infants: pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. , 1999, Developmental psychology.

[4]  Jeffrey M. Woodbridge Econometric Analysis of Cross Section and Panel Data , 2002 .

[5]  B. Efron,et al.  The Jackknife: The Bootstrap and Other Resampling Plans. , 1983 .

[6]  R. Martorell,et al.  Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries , 2013, The Lancet.

[7]  S. Arifeen,et al.  Appropriate infant feeding practices result in better growth of infants and young children in rural Bangladesh. , 2008, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[8]  S. El Arifeen,et al.  Effects of prenatal food and micronutrient supplementation on infant development: a randomized trial from the Maternal and Infant Nutrition Interventions, Matlab (MINIMat) study. , 2008, The American journal of clinical nutrition.

[9]  Christina H. Paxson,et al.  Cognitive Development Among Young Children in Ecuador: The Roles of Wealth, Health, and Parenting , 2005 .

[10]  S. Cueto,et al.  Psychometric characteristics of cognitive development and achievement instruments in Round 2 of Young Lives , 2009 .

[11]  Psychologie Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence , 2011, Definitions.

[12]  J. Colombo Infant Cognition: Predicting Later Intellectual Functioning , 1993 .

[13]  Kathy Sylva,et al.  The Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment Revisited. , 2004, Child and adolescent mental health.

[14]  R. Martorell,et al.  Nutritional supplementation in early childhood, schooling, and intellectual functioning in adulthood: a prospective study in Guatemala. , 2008, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[15]  E. Hair,et al.  Disparities in early learning and development: Lessons from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study - Birth Cohort (ECLS-B): (571822009-001) , 2009 .

[16]  P. Gertler,et al.  Socioeconomic gradients in child development in very young children: Evidence from India, Indonesia, Peru, and Senegal , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  D. Filmer,et al.  Cognitive Development among Young Children in Low-Income Countries , 2011 .

[18]  Emma Pitchforth,et al.  Development of a proxy wealth index for women utilizing emergency obstetric care in Bangladesh. , 2007, Health policy and planning.

[19]  M. Bottai,et al.  Critical windows of exposure for arsenic-associated impairment of cognitive function in pre-school girls and boys: a population-based cohort study. , 2011, International journal of epidemiology.

[20]  Martin W. Bloem,et al.  Effect of parental formal education on risk of child stunting in Indonesia and Bangladesh: a cross-sectional study , 2008, The Lancet.

[21]  H. Pan,et al.  WHO child growth standards: length/height-for-age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age , 2006 .

[22]  S. West,et al.  A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[23]  P. Engle,et al.  Strategies for reducing inequalities and improving developmental outcomes for young children in low-income and middle-income countries , 2011, The Lancet.

[24]  G. Fuchs,et al.  Zinc supplementation during pregnancy and effects on mental development and behaviour of infants: a follow-up study , 2002, The Lancet.

[25]  E. Galasso,et al.  Socioeconomic gradients and child development in a very low income population: evidence from Madagascar. , 2011, Developmental science.

[26]  M. Smrkovsky,et al.  Bayley Scales of Infant Development (BSID) - II. Nederlandse versie , 2002 .

[27]  G. Psacharopoulos Returns to investment in education: A global update , 1994 .

[28]  B. Efron The jackknife, the bootstrap, and other resampling plans , 1987 .

[29]  J. Shonkoff,et al.  Building a new biodevelopmental framework to guide the future of early childhood policy. , 2010, Child development.

[30]  P. Glewwe,et al.  Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries , 2007, The Lancet.

[31]  Brent R. Moulton Random group effects and the precision of regression estimates , 1986 .

[32]  C. Nelson,et al.  Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development , 2011, The Lancet.

[33]  J. Schafer,et al.  Missing data: our view of the state of the art. , 2002, Psychological methods.

[34]  Jacob Cohen,et al.  Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences , 1979 .

[35]  P. Engle,et al.  Use of Family Care Indicators and Their Relationship with Child Development in Bangladesh , 2010, Journal of health, population, and nutrition.

[36]  A. Barros,et al.  Child development in a birth cohort: effect of child stimulation is stronger in less educated mothers. , 2010, International journal of epidemiology.

[37]  R. Martorell,et al.  Influence of prenatal and postnatal growth on intellectual functioning in school-aged children. , 2012, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[38]  R. Martorell,et al.  Weight Gain in the First Two Years of Life Is an Important Predictor of Schooling Outcomes in Pooled Analyses from Five Birth Cohorts from Low- and Middle-Income Countries12 , 2010, The Journal of nutrition.