A selectionist approach to reinforcement.
暂无分享,去创建一个
J W Donahoe | J E Burgos | D C Palmer | J. Donahoe | J. Burgos | D. Palmer | D. C. Palmer
[1] B. Skinner. The Generic Nature of the Concepts of Stimulus and Response , 1935 .
[2] Kehoe Ej. Connectionist models of conditioning: A tutorial. , 1989 .
[3] J. Nevin. Overall matching versus momentary maximizing: Nevin (1969) revisited. , 1979 .
[4] B. F. Skinner,et al. A second type of superstition in the pigeon. , 1957 .
[5] I. Gormezano,et al. Effects of cocaine on conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response , 1991, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.
[6] J. Long. Elicitation and Reinforcement as Separate Stimulus Functions , 1966, Psychological reports.
[7] R. Rescorla,et al. Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.
[8] J. Donahoe,et al. Essentialism and selectionism in cognitive science and behavior analysis. , 1992, The American psychologist.
[9] James L. McClelland,et al. Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .
[10] B. Skinner. Superstition in the pigeon. , 1948, Journal of experimental psychology.
[11] K. Breland,et al. The misbehavior of organisms. , 1961 .
[12] A. Catania. Some Darwinian lessons for behavior analysis: A review of Bowler's The Eclipse of Darwinism1. , 1987 .
[13] C. Heth. Levels of aggregation and the generalized matching law. , 1992, Psychological review.
[14] W. Singer,et al. Selection of intrinsic horizontal connections in the visual cortex by correlated neuronal activity. , 1992, Science.
[15] H. M. Hanson. Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[16] A. Keller,et al. Long-term potentiation in the motor cortex. , 1989, Science.
[17] L. Squire. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.
[18] T. Bliss,et al. Long‐lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path , 1973, The Journal of physiology.
[19] B. Skinner. Two Types of Conditioned Reflex: A Reply to Konorski and Miller , 1937 .
[20] M. Sidman,et al. Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: an expansion of the testing paradigm. , 1982, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[21] L. Swanson,et al. The projections of the ventral tegmental area and adjacent regions: A combined fluorescent retrograde tracer and immunofluorescence study in the rat , 1982, Brain Research Bulletin.
[22] J. Hinson,et al. Hill-climbing by pigeons. , 1983, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[23] E. Kehoe. A layered network model of associative learning: learning to learn and configuration. , 1988, Psychological review.
[24] B. Skinner. Two Types of Conditioned Reflex and a Pseudo Type , 1935 .
[25] K. Stickney,et al. Attenuation of blocking by a change in US locus , 1983 .
[26] Jer Staddon,et al. Optimization: a result or a mechanism? , 1983 .
[27] J. Panksepp,et al. An incentive model of rewarding brain stimulation. , 1969, Psychological review.
[28] D. R. Thomas,et al. STIMULUS GENERALIZATION OF A POSITIVE CONDITIONED REINFORCER. II. EFFECTS OF DISCRIMINATION TRAINING. , 1964, Journal of experimental psychology.
[29] W Vaughan,et al. Melioration, matching, and maximization. , 1981, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[30] David C. Palmer,et al. THE INTERPRETATION OF COMPLEX HUMAN BEHAVIOR: SOME REACTIONS TO PARALLEL DISTRIBUTED PROCESSING, EDITED BY J. L. McCLELLAND, D. E. RUMELHART, AND THE PDP RESEARCH GROUP1 , 1989 .
[31] D. R. Williams,et al. Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. , 1969, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[32] B. Skinner,et al. Giving up the ghost , 1981, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[33] J. Donahoe,et al. Paranoid schizophrenia may be caused by dopamine hyperactivity of CA1 hippocampus , 1992, Biological Psychiatry.
[34] Enduring problems for molecular accounts of operant behavior. , 1990 .
[35] C. B. Woodbury. The learning of stimulus patterns by dogs. , 1943 .
[36] R. Beninger. The role of dopamine in locomotor activity and learning , 1983, Brain Research Reviews.
[37] H. M. Jenkins,et al. Blocking the development of stimulus control , 1970 .
[38] L. Stein,et al. Cellular investigations of behavioral reinforcement , 1989, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[39] R. Wise,et al. A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction. , 1987, Psychological review.
[40] J. Dinsmoor. A quantitative comparison of the discriminative and reinforcing functions of a stimulus. , 1950, Journal of experimental psychology.
[41] I. Gormezano,et al. EFFECT OF HALOPERIDOL (HAL) AND PIMOZIDE (PIM) ON PAVLOVIAN CONDITIONING OF THE RABBIT NICTITATING MEMBRANE RESPONSE , 1979 .
[42] C. Shimp. Optimal behavior in free-operant experiments. , 1969 .
[43] J. Pear,et al. The operant-respondent distinction: Future directions. , 1984, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[44] W. H. Morse,et al. Schedules using noxious stimuli. III. Responding maintained with response-produced electric shocks. , 1968, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[45] A. Silberberg,et al. The Structure of Choice , 1978 .
[46] R. Herrnstein. On the law of effect. , 1970, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[47] J. Yeomans. Two substrates for medial forebrain bundle self-stimulation: Myelinated axons and dopamine axons , 1989, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[48] R. Gehrz,et al. The Formation of Stellar Systems from Interstellar Molecular Clouds , 1984, Science.