Oculomotor Function in Chronic Traumatic Brain Injury

ObjectiveTo characterize oculomotor function using visually guided saccade and antisaccade (AS) tasks in chronic traumatic brain injury (TBI) and assess the relationship to neuropsychologic testing. BackgroundTBI causes dysfunction of prefrontal cortex, in part by disrupting cortical and subcortical pathways, resulting in specific cognitive impairments. Oculomotor function tests provide a method of assessing the integrity of these pathways. MethodsTwenty mild TBI (MTBI), 17 moderate to severe TBI (M/STBI), and 19 healthy controls underwent oculomotor and neuropsychologic testing. ResultsOn the visually guided saccade task, the M/STBI showed longer latencies and reduced accuracy. On the AS task, which is more dependent on prefrontal cortex function, both patient groups committed more prosaccade errors than controls. On neuropsychologic testing, only the M/STBI patients were significantly impaired. Correlations were found between AS and neuropsychologic performance. ConclusionsThe M/STBI group was impaired on both oculomotor tasks and neuropsychologic testing, consistent with more global neuropathology. The MTBI group showed impaired performance primarily on the AS task, consistent with prefrontal system dysfunction. Hence, oculomotor testing is sensitive to the range of neuropathology in chronic TBI, and importantly, may be more sensitive to neuropathology in MTBI.

[1]  Jeanyung Chey,et al.  Spatial working memory span, delayed response and executive function in schizophrenia , 2002, Psychiatry Research.

[2]  Jelle Jolles,et al.  The Stroop Color-Word Test , 2006, Assessment.

[3]  S. Pompéia,et al.  A Comparative Study of Performance in the Conners' Continuous Performance Test Between Brazilian and North American Children , 2008, Journal of attention disorders.

[4]  F. Servadei,et al.  A systematic review of brain injury epidemiology in Europe , 2006, Acta Neurochirurgica.

[5]  R. Wurtz,et al.  The Neurobiology of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1989 .

[6]  J. Adams,et al.  Diffuse axonal injury and traumatic coma in the primate , 1982, Annals of neurology.

[7]  E. Zackai,et al.  Effects of a functional COMT polymorphism on prefrontal cognitive function in patients with 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. , 2004, The American journal of psychiatry.

[8]  C R Genovese,et al.  Cortical networks subserving pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans: An FMRI study , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[9]  D. Delis,et al.  The California verbal learning test , 2016 .

[10]  A. Hartley,et al.  Further evidence that negative priming in the Stroop color-word task is equivalent in older and younger adults. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[11]  D. Gronwall Paced Auditory Serial-Addition Task: A Measure of Recovery from Concussion , 1977, Perceptual and motor skills.

[12]  F. Newcombe,et al.  The psychological effects of concussion D. M. A. Gronwall & H. Sampson, Auckland University Press/Oxford University Press (1974). 118 pp , 1976, Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  J. Lani,et al.  Conners' Continuous Performance Test II (CPT II) , 2002 .

[15]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[16]  Richard D. Jones,et al.  Eye movement and visuomotor arm movement deficits following mild closed head injury. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  H. Levin,et al.  Serial MRI and neurobehavioural findings after mild to moderate closed head injury. , 1992, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[18]  Grace Scott,et al.  Brain damage in fatal non-missile head injury. , 1980 .

[19]  M. Kosmidis,et al.  Relationship between psychopathology and cognitive functioning in schizophrenia. , 2004, Comprehensive psychiatry.

[20]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  The Role of the Human Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex in Ocular Motor Behavior , 2005, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[21]  A. Jensen,et al.  The Stroop color-word test: a review. , 1966, Acta psychologica.

[22]  O. Godefroy,et al.  Frontal syndrome and disorders of executive functions , 2003, Journal of Neurology.

[23]  Christie Golden,et al.  Stroop Color and Word Test: Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. , 1978 .

[24]  E. Miller,et al.  THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX AND COGNITIVE CONTROL , 2000 .

[25]  J. Bazarian,et al.  Mild traumatic brain injury in the United States, 1998–2000 , 2005, Brain injury.

[26]  Z. Groswasser,et al.  Impersistent execution of saccadic eye movements after traumatic brain injury. , 1995, Brain injury.

[27]  H S Levin,et al.  The Galveston Orientation and Amnesia Test: A Practical Scale to Assess Cognition after Head Injury , 1979, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[28]  J. Povlishock,et al.  Traumatically Induced Axonal Injury: Pathogenesis and Pathobiological Implications , 1991, Brain pathology.

[29]  H. Levin,et al.  The frontal lobes and traumatic brain injury. , 1994, The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences.

[30]  A. Rey Lexamen psychologique : Dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique (Les problemes) , 1941 .

[31]  J. Borg,et al.  Summary of the WHO Collaborating Centre for Neurotrauma Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. , 2005, Journal of rehabilitation medicine.

[32]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[33]  R. Bornstein,et al.  Effects of Past Noninjection Drug Abuse Upon Executive Function and Working Memory in HIV Infection , 2003, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[34]  Sharlene D. Newman,et al.  Frontal and parietal participation in problem solving in the Tower of London: fMRI and computational modeling of planning and high-level perception , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  Cameron S Carter,et al.  Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes. , 2005, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[36]  L. Crevits,et al.  Antisaccades and remembered saccades in mild traumatic brain injury , 2000, Journal of Neurology.

[37]  R. Paul,et al.  Evaluating elements of executive functioning as predictors of instrumental activities of daily living (IADLs). , 2006, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[38]  M. Lovell,et al.  Does loss of consciousness predict neuropsychological decrements after concussion? , 1999, Clinical journal of sport medicine : official journal of the Canadian Academy of Sport Medicine.

[39]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  The Prefrontal Substrate of Reflexive Saccade Inhibition in Humans , 2005, Biological Psychiatry.

[40]  W. Hauser,et al.  The Epidemiology of Traumatic Brain Injury: A Review , 2003, Epilepsia.

[41]  A. Jensen,et al.  Scoring the Stroop test. , 1965, Acta psychologica.

[42]  B. J. McCurtain,et al.  Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  J. Borg,et al.  Incidence, risk factors and prevention of mild traumatic brain injury: results of the WHO Collaborating Centre Task Force on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. , 2004, Journal of rehabilitation medicine.

[44]  Freddie H Fu,et al.  The “Value Added” of Neurocognitive Testing after Sports-Related Concussion , 2006, The American journal of sports medicine.

[45]  T. Shallice Specific impairments of planning. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[46]  K. Cicerone,et al.  Definition of mild traumatic brain injury , 1993 .

[47]  A. Vandierendonck,et al.  Gap Effect in Reflexive and Intentional Prosaccades , 2005, Neuropsychobiology.

[48]  R. Reitan Validity of the Trail Making Test as an Indicator of Organic Brain Damage , 1958 .