Decision value computation in DLPFC and VMPFC adjusts to the available decision time

It is increasingly clear that simple decisions are made by computing decision values for the options under consideration, and then comparing these values to make a choice. Computational models of this process suggest that it involves the accumulation of information over time, but little is known about the temporal course of valuation in the brain. To examine this, we manipulated the available decision time and observed the consequences in the brain and behavioral correlates of choice. Participants were scanned with functional magnetic resonance imaging while they chose to eat or not eat basic food items, in two conditions differing in the amount of time provided for choice. After identifying valuation‐related regions with unbiased whole‐brain general linear models, we analyzed two regions of interest: ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). Finite impulse response models of the upsampled estimated neural activity from those regions allowed us to examine the onset, duration and termination of decision value signals, and to compare across regions. We found evidence for the immediate onset of value computation in both regions, but an extended duration with longer decision time. However, this was not accompanied by behavioral changes in either the accuracy or determinants of choice. Finally, there was modest evidence that DLPFC computation correlated with, but lagged behind, VMPFC computation, suggesting the sharing of information across these regions. These findings have important implications for models of decision value computation and choice.

[1]  M. Alexander,et al.  Principles of Neural Science , 1981 .

[2]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Changing your mind: a computational mechanism of vacillation , 2009, Nature.

[3]  E. Gilder,et al.  The Authors , 1977 .

[4]  Antonio Rangel,et al.  Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system , 2009, NeuroImage.

[6]  Ian Krajbich,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[7]  Timothy D. Hanks,et al.  Bounded Integration in Parietal Cortex Underlies Decisions Even When Viewing Duration Is Dictated by the Environment , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Dynamic Construction of Stimulus Values in the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex , 2011, PloS one.

[9]  A. Rangel,et al.  Dissociating valuation and saliency signals during decision-making. , 2011, Cerebral cortex.

[10]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Dissociating the Role of the Orbitofrontal Cortex and the Striatum in the Computation of Goal Values and Prediction Errors , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[11]  Samuel M. McClure,et al.  Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards , 2004, Science.

[12]  R Turner,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[13]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The physics of optimal decision making: a formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. , 2006, Psychological review.

[14]  C. Padoa-Schioppa Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. , 2011, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[16]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Prefrontal Coding of Temporally Discounted Values during Intertemporal Choice , 2008, Neuron.

[17]  J. Gold,et al.  Neural correlates of perceptual decision making before, during, and after decision commitment in monkey frontal eye field. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  A multivariate analysis of PET activation studies , 1996, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Colin Camerer,et al.  Transformation of stimulus value signals into motor commands during simple choice , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  E. Miller,et al.  Neuronal activity in primate dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortex during performance of a reward preference task , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[21]  M. Rushworth,et al.  Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right "temporoparietal junction area": evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  Eric J. Johnson,et al.  Lateral prefrontal cortex and self-control in intertemporal choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex: compensation of susceptibility-induced gradients in the readout direction , 2007, Magnetic Resonance Materials in Physics, Biology and Medicine.

[24]  J. Gold,et al.  Banburismus and the Brain Decoding the Relationship between Sensory Stimuli, Decisions, and Reward , 2002, Neuron.

[25]  F. Christian How the Brain Integrates Costs and Benefits During Decision Making , 2010 .

[26]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Orbitofrontal Cortex Encodes Willingness to Pay in Everyday Economic Transactions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Representation of Confidence Associated with a Decision by Neurons in the Parietal Cortex , 2009, Science.

[28]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Review Frontal Cortex and Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Figure 1. Frontal Brain Regions in the Macaque Involved in Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Finer Grained Anatomical Divisions with Frontal Cortical Systems for Reward-guided Behavior , 2022 .

[29]  H. Johansen-Berg,et al.  Distinct and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting state functional connectivity. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[30]  D. Wilkin,et al.  Neuron , 2001, Brain Research.

[31]  Christof Koch,et al.  The Drift Diffusion Model Can Account for the Accuracy and Reaction Time of Value-Based Choices Under High and Low Time Pressure , 2010, Judgment and Decision Making.

[32]  S. Kennerley,et al.  Heterogeneous reward signals in prefrontal cortex , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[33]  J. Malmaud,et al.  Focusing Attention on the Health Aspects of Foods Changes Value Signals in vmPFC and Improves Dietary Choice , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Appetitive and Aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision-making , 2009, NeuroImage.

[35]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Diffusion-Weighted Imaging Tractography-Based Parcellation of the Human Parietal Cortex and Comparison with Human and Macaque Resting-State Functional Connectivity , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  Christof Koch,et al.  Consumers can make decisions in as little as a third of a second , 2011, Judgment and Decision Making.

[37]  E. Fehr,et al.  Dorsolateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortex orchestrate normative choice , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  P. Glimcher,et al.  The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution , 2003, NeuroImage.