When Time Shapes Behavior: fMRI Evidence of Brain Correlates of Temporal Monitoring

Time processing may shape behavior in several ways, although the underlying neural correlates are still poorly understood. When preparatory intervals between stimuli vary randomly in a block, for instance, responses are faster as the interval gets longer. This effect, known as variable foreperiod (FP) effect, has been attributed to a process monitoring the conditional probability of stimulus occurrence as the interval increases. Previous evidence points to the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) as a possible node for this time-monitoring process. The present study addresses this hypothesis with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Block-design fMRI was used on 14 young participants while they performed a visual discrimination task with fixed and variable preparatory intervals (FPs) of 1 and 3 sec. In the variable versus fixed FP contrast, the right DLPFC and a visual area were more activated in the subgroup of participants who showed a reliable variable FP effect than in another subgroup who did not show that effect. Only the activation in the right DLPFC was supported by a significant interaction between FP condition (variable vs. fixed) and group. This finding may reflect possible differences in the strategy adopted by the two subgroups of participants while performing the task. Although results suggest that many brain areas may be involved in preparation over time, the role of the right DLPFC is critical to observe the strategically mediated behavioral effects in the variable FP paradigm.

[1]  M. Phillips,et al.  Interacting outcome retrieval, anticipation, and feedback processes in the human brain. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[2]  J. Lupiáñez,et al.  Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials , 2006, Brain Research.

[3]  C. Brunia,et al.  Wait and see. , 2001, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[4]  Florin Dolcos,et al.  Similarities and Differences in the Neural Correlates of Episodic Memory Retrieval and Working Memory , 2002, NeuroImage.

[5]  A. Baumeister,et al.  Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency , 1969 .

[6]  T. Shallice,et al.  Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[7]  W. Meck,et al.  Neuroanatomical and Neurochemical Substrates of Timing , 2011, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[8]  D. Yves von Cramon,et al.  Variants of uncertainty in decision-making and their neural correlates , 2005, Brain Research Bulletin.

[9]  R. Miall,et al.  Brain activation patterns during measurement of sub- and supra-second intervals , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  J Richard Simon,et al.  Differential effects of a foreperiod countdown procedure on simple and choice reaction time. , 1975, Journal of motor behavior.

[11]  C D Frith,et al.  The functional roles of prefrontal cortex in episodic memory. II. Retrieval. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  G. Madison,et al.  Variability in isochronous tapping: higher order dependencies as a function of intertap interval. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  R M Church,et al.  Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[14]  J A Michon,et al.  A dynamic model of timing behavior. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[15]  S. Los,et al.  Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  J. Requin,et al.  The effect of conditional probability of the response signal on the simple reaction time. , 1969, Acta psychologica.

[17]  R. Ulrich,et al.  Response force is sensitive to the temporal uncertainty of response stimuli , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[18]  H. Woodrow The measurement of attention , 1914 .

[19]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj097 Role of Prefrontal and Anterior Cingulate Regions in Decision-Making Processes Shared by Memory and Nonmemory Tasks , 2005 .

[20]  W. H. Teichner Recent studies of simple reaction time. , 1954, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Role of the prefrontal cortex in the foreperiod effect: TMS evidence for dual mechanisms in temporal preparation. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[22]  T Shallice,et al.  Regional frontal injuries cause distinct impairments in cognitive control , 2007, Neurology.

[23]  J. Alegria Sequential Effects of Foreperiod Duration as a function of the Frequency of Foreperiod Repetitions. , 1975, Journal of motor behavior.

[24]  K. Zilles,et al.  Fast reaction to different sensory modalities activates common fields in the motor areas, but the anterior cingulate cortex is involved in the speed of reaction. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[25]  Tim Shallice,et al.  Specific impairments of rule induction in different frontal lobe subgroups , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[26]  R. Davis,et al.  The Role of “Attention” in the Psychological Refractory Period , 1959 .

[27]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Prefrontal involvement in source memory: An electrophysiological investigation of accounts concerning confidence and accuracy , 2006, Brain Research.

[28]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Temporal preparation in aging: A functional MRI study , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  Jan Derrfuss,et al.  Cognitive control in the posterior frontolateral cortex: evidence from common activations in task coordination, interference control, and working memory , 2004, NeuroImage.

[30]  Todd B. Parrish,et al.  The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention , 2003, NeuroImage.

[31]  Adrian R. Willoughby,et al.  The Medial Frontal Cortex and the Rapid Processing of Monetary Gains and Losses , 2002, Science.

[32]  T. Robbins,et al.  A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[33]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  C. V. D. Heuvel,et al.  Intentional and unintentional contributions of nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. , 2001 .

[35]  E. T. Klemmer Simple reaction time as a function of time uncertainty. , 1957, Journal of experimental psychology.

[36]  L. Green,et al.  Prefrontal brain activity predicts temporally extended decision-making behavior. , 2005, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[37]  T Shallice,et al.  Impaired concentration due to frontal lobe damage from two distinct lesion sites , 2005, Neurology.

[38]  Juan Lupiáñez,et al.  Temporal orienting deficit after prefrontal damage. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  D. Stuss,et al.  Organizational strategies with unilateral or bilateral frontal lobe injury in word learning tasks. , 1994 .

[40]  D. Stuss,et al.  The effects of focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal fluency , 1998, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[41]  A. Nobre,et al.  The hazards of time , 2007, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[42]  E. A. Thomas Reaction-time studies: the anticipation and interaction of responses. , 1967, The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology.

[43]  Brenda Milner,et al.  Differential effects of frontal-lobe lesions on cognitive estimation and spatial memory , 1984, Neuropsychologia.

[44]  R. C. Oldfield THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .

[45]  L. Karlin Reaction time as a function of foreperiod duration and variability. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.

[46]  Jeffrey S. Maxwell,et al.  Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortical Activity and Behavioral Inhibition , 2009, Psychological science.

[47]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Overrecruitment in the Aging Brain as a Function of Task Demands: Evidence for a Compensatory View , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  D. Schacter,et al.  False recognition and the right frontal lobe: A case study , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[49]  T. Paus Primate anterior cingulate cortex: Where motor control, drive and cognition interface , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[50]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  Sequential Activation of Human Oculomotor Centers During Planning of Visually-Guided Eye Movements: A Combined fMRI-MEG Study , 2007, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[51]  R. Gottsdanker Uncertainty, timekeeping, and simple reaction time. , 1970, Journal of motor behavior.

[52]  R. Elliott Simple reaction time: effects associated with age, preparatory interval, incentive-shift, and mode of presentation. , 1970, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[53]  Guy Marchal,et al.  FMRI Studies of the Supplementary Motor Area and the Premotor Cortex , 1997, NeuroImage.

[54]  Anthony R. McIntosh,et al.  Task-Independent Effect of Time on rCBF , 1998, NeuroImage.

[55]  R Näätänen,et al.  The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. , 1970, Acta psychologica.

[56]  Alick Elithorn,et al.  Central Inhibition—Some Refractory Observations , 1955 .

[57]  A. Nobre,et al.  Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[58]  R W Cox,et al.  AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. , 1996, Computers and biomedical research, an international journal.

[59]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[60]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Foreperiod and simple reaction time. , 1981 .

[61]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  Decay of Stimulus Spatial Code in Horizontal and Vertical Simon Tasks , 2009, The Journal of general psychology.

[62]  Sara Torriero,et al.  Underestimation of time perception after repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation , 2003, Neurology.

[63]  T. Shallice,et al.  Multiple frontal systems controlling response speed , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[64]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[65]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[66]  A. Sanders Expectancy: application and measurement. , 1966, Acta psychologica.

[67]  G. de Marco,et al.  Alertness in young healthy subjects: An fMRI study of brain region interactivity enhanced by a warning signal , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[68]  Malcolm A Binns,et al.  Dissociations within the anterior attentional system: effects of task complexity and irrelevant information on reaction time speed and accuracy. , 2002, Neuropsychology.

[69]  Antonino Vallesi,et al.  The neural basis of temporal preparation: Insights from brain tumor patients , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[70]  C. Caltagirone,et al.  Repetitive TMS of cerebellum interferes with millisecond time processing , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[71]  R S Nickerson,et al.  Expectancy, waiting time and the psychological refractory period. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[72]  I. Stilitz,et al.  Conditional Probability and Components of RT in the Variable Foreperiod Experiment , 1972, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[73]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Selection for Cognitive Control: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study on the Selection of Task-Relevant Information , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[74]  M. Botvinick Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[75]  Terence W. Picton,et al.  Multiple Effects of Prefrontal Lesions on Task-Switching , 2007, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[76]  Rolf Ulrich,et al.  The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[77]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Neural Correlates of Memory Retrieval during Recognition Memory and Cued Recall , 1998, NeuroImage.

[78]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Functional MRI studies of spatial and nonspatial working memory. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[79]  R. Dolan,et al.  Encoding and retrieval in human medial temporal lobes: An empirical investigation using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) , 1999, Hippocampus.

[80]  T. Shallice,et al.  A Multidisciplinary Approach to Anterior Attentional Functions a , 1995, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.