Bottlenose dolphins’ broadband clicks are structured for communication

Bottlenose dolphins’ broadband click vocalizations are well studied in the literature with respect to their echolocation function. Their use for communication among conspecifics has long been speculated, but not conclusively established so far. In this study we categorize dolphins’ click productions into types on the basis of their amplitude contour and analyze the distribution of individual clicks and click sequences against their duration and length. We demonstrate that the repertoire and composition of clicks and click sequences follow three key linguistic laws of efficient communication, namely, Zipf’s rank-frequency law, the law of brevity and Menzerath-Altmann law. Conforming to the rank-frequency law suggests that clicks may form a linguistic code that is subject to selective pressures for unification, on the one hand, and diversification, on the other. Conforming to the other two laws also implies that dolphins use clicks in accord with the compression criterion, or minimization of code length without loss of information. Our results furnish novel evidence for conformity to the linguistic laws in this type of dolphins’ signal and in the realm of animal vocalizations more generally.

[1]  M. Magnasco,et al.  The Relevance of Human Whistled Languages for the Analysis and Decoding of Dolphin Communication , 2021, Frontiers in Psychology.

[2]  Morgan L. Gustison,et al.  Linguistic laws in biology. , 2021, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[3]  Leonid M. Martyushev,et al.  Principle of Least Effort and Sentence Length in Public Speaking , 2021, Entropy.

[4]  Brittany L. Jones,et al.  Sounds produced by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops): a review of the defining characteristics and acoustic criteria of the dolphin vocal repertoire , 2020, Bioacoustics.

[5]  Holger Klinck,et al.  Brevity is not a universal in animal communication: evidence for compression depends on the unit of analysis in small ape vocalizations , 2020, Royal Society Open Science.

[6]  David Wheatcroft,et al.  Call combinations in birds and the evolution of compositional syntax , 2018, PLoS biology.

[7]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Trade-offs in the production of animal vocal sequences: insights from the structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots , 2017, Frontiers in Zoology.

[8]  Vyacheslav A. Ryabov,et al.  The study of acoustic signals and the supposed spoken language of the dolphins , 2016 .

[9]  Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho,et al.  Gelada vocal sequences follow Menzerath’s linguistic law , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  D. Bates,et al.  Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4 , 2015 .

[11]  S. Piantadosi Zipf’s word frequency law in natural language: A critical review and future directions , 2014, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[12]  David Lusseau,et al.  Compression as a Universal Principle of Animal Behavior , 2013, Cogn. Sci..

[13]  John A. Hildebrand,et al.  The behavioral context of common dolphin (Delphinus sp.) vocalizations , 2012 .

[14]  Ulrich Bodenhofer,et al.  APCluster: an R package for affinity propagation clustering , 2011, Bioinform..

[15]  A. Radford,et al.  Brevity is not always a virtue in primate communication , 2011, Biology Letters.

[16]  Ramon Ferrer-i-Cancho,et al.  The self-organization of genomes , 2010, Complex..

[17]  Diana Reiss,et al.  Quantitative Comparison of Whistle Repertoires from Captive Adult Bottlenose Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops truncatus): a Re‐evaluation of the Signature Whistle Hypothesis , 2010 .

[18]  B. McCowan A New Quantitative Technique for Categorizing Whistles Using Simulated Signals and Whistles from Captive Bottlenose Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops truncatus) , 2010 .

[19]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Campbell's monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  Delbert Dueck,et al.  Clustering by Passing Messages Between Data Points , 2007, Science.

[21]  J. Weijer,et al.  Word length, sentence length and frequency: Zipf revisited , 2004 .

[22]  LAURANCE R. DOYLE,et al.  Quantitative tools for comparing animal communication systems: information theory applied to bottlenose dolphin whistle repertoires , 1999, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  T. Cranford,et al.  Functional morphology and homology in the odontocete nasal complex: Implications for sound generation , 1996, Journal of morphology.

[24]  Whitlow W. L. Au,et al.  The Sonar of Dolphins , 1993, Springer New York.

[25]  Siswadi,et al.  Selecting among weibull, lognormal and gamma distributions using complete and censored smaples , 1982 .

[26]  Alaa A. Kharbouch,et al.  Three models for the description of language , 1956, IRE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[27]  C. B. Williams A note on the statistical analysis of sentence-length as a criterion of literary style , 1940 .

[28]  R. Paget The Origin of Speech , 1927, Nature.

[29]  Heather Ruth Spence,et al.  The Importance of Bioacoustics for Dolphin Welfare: Soundscape Characterization with Implications for Management , 2015 .

[30]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[31]  Vincent M. Janik,et al.  Chapter 4 Acoustic Communication in Delphinids , 2009 .

[32]  C. B. Williams A NOTE ON THE STATISTICAL ANALYSIS OF SENTENCE-LENGTH AS A CRITERION OF LITERARY STYLE BY , 2008 .

[33]  Ted W. Cranford,et al.  In Search of Impulse Sound Sources in Odontocetes , 2000 .

[34]  H. Kucera,et al.  Computational analysis of present-day American English , 1967 .

[35]  George Kingsley Zipf,et al.  Human behavior and the principle of least effort , 1949 .