Role of the human supplementary eye field in the control of saccadic eye movements

The precise function of the supplementary eye field (SEF) is poorly understood. Although electrophysiological and functional imaging studies are important for demonstrating when SEF neurones are active, lesion studies are critical to establish the functions for which the SEF is essential. Here we report a series of investigations performed on an extremely rare individual with a highly focal lesion of the medial frontal cortex. High-resolution structural imaging demonstrated that his lesion was confined to the region of the left paracentral sulcus, the anatomical locus of the SEF. Behavioural testing revealed that the patient was significantly impaired when required to switch between anti- and pro-saccades, when there were conflicting rules governing stimulus–response mappings for saccades. Similarly, the results of an arbitrary stimulus–response associative learning task demonstrated that he was impaired when required to select the appropriate saccade from conflicting eye movement responses, but not for limb movements on an analogous manual task. When making memory-guided saccadic sequences, the patient demonstrated hypometria, like patients with Parkinson's disease, but had no significant difficulties in reproducing the order of saccades correctly on a task that emphasized accuracy with a wide temporal segregation between responses. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the SEF plays a key role in implementing control when there is conflict between several, ongoing competing saccadic responses, but not when eye movements need to be made accurately in sequence.

[1]  M. Inase,et al.  Selective coding of motor sequence in the supplementary motor area of the monkey cerebral cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  Michael W. Cole,et al.  Canceling planned action: an FMRI study of countermanding saccades. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  J B Poline,et al.  Human cortical networks for new and familiar sequences of saccades. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  E. J. Tehovnik,et al.  Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates , 2000, Brain Research Reviews.

[5]  Joshua W. Brown,et al.  Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes , 2002, Neuron.

[6]  J Tanji,et al.  Supplementary motor cortex in organization of movement. , 1996, European neurology.

[7]  D. Levy,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[8]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation over the region of the supplementary motor area during sequences of memory-guided saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  Leslie Henderson,et al.  Eye movements and spatial working memory inParkinsons disease , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  S. Wise,et al.  Supplementary eye field contrasted with the frontal eye field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Liana Machado,et al.  Control of eye movement reflexes , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  J. Schall,et al.  Performance monitoring by the supplementary eye ® eld , 2000 .

[13]  J. Lynch,et al.  Slow and saccadic eye movements evoked by microstimulation in the supplementary eye field of the cebus monkey. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Distributed but convergent ordering of corticostriatal projections: analysis of the frontal eye field and the supplementary eye field in the macaque monkey , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  M Missal,et al.  Facilitation of smooth pursuit initiation by electrical stimulation in the supplementary eye fields. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  J Schlag,et al.  Reward-predicting and reward-detecting neuronal activity in the primate supplementary eye field. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Evidence for a supplementary eye field. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  C. Curtis,et al.  Success and Failure Suppressing Reflexive Behavior , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  M. Liu,et al.  Single-neuron activity in the dorsomedial frontal cortex during smooth-pursuit eye movements to predictable target motion , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[20]  P. Tobler,et al.  Role of human frontal and supplementary eye fields in double step saccades , 2002, Neuroreport.

[21]  K. Arbuthnott,et al.  Trail Making Test, Part B as a Measure of Executive Control: Validation Using a Set-Switching Paradigm , 2000, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[22]  S P Wise,et al.  Conditional oculomotor learning: population vectors in the supplementary eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  B. Fischer,et al.  Characteristics of “anti” saccades in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  S. Wise,et al.  Evolution of Directional Preferences in the Supplementary Eye Field during Acquisition of Conditional Oculomotor Associations , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[26]  N. Shimizu [Neurology of eye movements]. , 2000, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[27]  A. Berthoz,et al.  PET study of voluntary saccadic eye movements in humans: basal ganglia-thalamocortical system and cingulate cortex involvement. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  S. Wise,et al.  Neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field during acquisition of conditional oculomotor associations. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Madeleine Schlag-Rey,et al.  Primate antisaccade. II. Supplementary eye field neuronal activity predicts correct performance. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  A. Berthoz,et al.  An anatomical landmark for the supplementary eye fields in human revealed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  Tony Ro,et al.  Modulation of antisaccades by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[33]  D Kömpf,et al.  Deficits of smooth pursuit eye movements after frontal and parietal lesions. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[34]  M. Roesch,et al.  Neuronal activity in macaque SEF and ACC during performance of tasks involving conflict. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  The predictability of saccadic latency in a novel voluntary oculomotor task , 1980, Vision Research.

[36]  K Shmueli,et al.  High resolution MRI of the brain at 4.7 Tesla using fast spin echo imaging. , 2003, The British journal of radiology.

[37]  Christopher Kennard,et al.  Reflexive, symbolic, and affective contributions to eye movements during task switching: response selection. , 2004, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.

[38]  J. Tanji,et al.  Contrasting neuronal activity in the supplementary and frontal eye fields during temporal organization of multiple saccades. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Self-control during response conflict by human supplementary eye field , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[41]  T Mergner,et al.  Saccadic reaction times in patients with frontal and parietal lesions. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  Valentino Bettinardi,et al.  Neural control of fast-regular saccades and antisaccades: an investigation using positron emission tomography , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  E. J. Tehovnik,et al.  Reversible inactivation of macaque dorsomedial frontal cortex: effects on saccades and fixations , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[44]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[45]  J. Tanji,et al.  Cellular activity in the supplementary eye field during sequential performance of multiple saccades. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  D. Stuss,et al.  Stroop performance in focal lesion patients: dissociation of processes and frontal lobe lesion location , 2001, Neuropsychologia.

[47]  Parashkev Nachev,et al.  Volition and Conflict in Human Medial Frontal Cortex , 2005, Current Biology.

[48]  S. Rivaud,et al.  Role of the left and right supplementary motor areas in memory‐guided saccade sequences , 1993, Annals of neurology.

[49]  B. J. McCurtain,et al.  Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. , 1998, Cerebral cortex.

[50]  H. Nelson A Modified Card Sorting Test Sensitive to Frontal Lobe Defects , 1976, Cortex.

[51]  L. Henderson,et al.  Eye movements and spatial working memory in Parkinson's disease. , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  M. Missal,et al.  Supplementary eye fields stimulation facilitates anticipatory pursuit. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[53]  M. Schlag-Rey,et al.  Antisaccade performance predicted by neuronal activity in the supplementary eye field , 1997, Nature.

[54]  T. Mergner,et al.  Relationship between saccadic eye movements and cortical activity as measured by fMRI: quantitative and qualitative aspects , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[55]  R. J. Seitz,et al.  Activation of frontoparietal cortices during memorized triple‐step sequences of saccadic eye movements: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[56]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Functional Anatomy of a Prelearned Sequence of Horizontal Saccades in Humans , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[57]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Movement sequence-related activity reflecting numerical order of components in supplementary and presupplementary motor areas. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Neuronal activity in medial frontal cortex during learning of sequential procedures. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[59]  C R Olson,et al.  Object-centered direction selectivity in the macaque supplementary eye field , 1995, Science.

[60]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[61]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  A Neural Correlate of Oculomotor Sequences in Supplementary Eye Field , 2002, Neuron.

[62]  S. Rivaud,et al.  Impairment of sequences of memory‐guided saccades after supplementary motor area lesions , 1990, Annals of neurology.