A heuristic mathematical model for the dynamics of sensory conflict and motion sickness.

The etiology of motion sickness is now usually explained in terms of a qualitatively formulated "sensory conflict" hypothesis. By consideration of the information processing task faced by the central nervous system in estimating body spatial orientation and in controlling active body movement using an "internal model" referenced control strategy, a mathematical model for sensory conflict generation is developed. The model incorporates and extends models proposed by von Holst, Held, and Reason, and is congruent with multisensory models for spatial orientation developed by Young and coworkers. The model postulates a major dynamic functional role for sensory conflict signals in movement control, as well as in sensory-motor adaptation. It accounts for the role of active movement in creating motion sickness symptoms in some experimental circumstances, and in alleviating them in others. The relationship between motion sickness produced by "sensory rearrangement" and that resulting from external motion disturbances is explicitly defined. A nonlinear conflict averaging model is proposed which describes dynamic aspects of experimentally observed subjective discomfort sensation, and suggests resulting behaviours. The model admits several possibilities for adaptive mechanisms which do not involve internal model updating. Further systematic efforts to experimentally refine and validate the model are indicated.

[1]  Y. J. Tejwani,et al.  A new concept , 1984 .

[2]  C M Oman,et al.  Dynamics of subjective discomfort in motion sickness as measured with a magnitude estimation method. , 1982, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[3]  S. A. Riedel A comprehensive system model for motion/space sickness. Preliminary results , 1982 .

[4]  K. Schaefer,et al.  ACOUSTIC‐INDUCED EYE MOVEMENTS * , 1981, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[5]  D. R. Jones,et al.  Biofeedback rehabilitation of airsick aircrew. , 1981, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[6]  V Henn,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction in motion perception and the generation of nystagmus. , 1980, Neurosciences Research Program bulletin.

[7]  C M Oman,et al.  Visually induced self-motion sensation adapts rapidly to left-right visual reversal. , 1980, Science.

[8]  J. Goldberg,et al.  Efferent vestibular system in the squirrel monkey: anatomical location and influence on afferent activity. , 1980, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[9]  J. Gyr,et al.  Motor-sensory feedback and geometry of visual space: an attempted replication , 1979 .

[10]  W Precht Vestibular mechanisms. , 1979, Annual review of neuroscience.

[11]  J T Reason,et al.  Motion Sickness Adaptation: A Neural Mismatch Model 1 , 1978, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.

[12]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Visual-Vestibular Interaction: Effects on Self-Motion Perception and Postural Control , 1978 .

[13]  A Graybiel,et al.  Bidirectional overadaptation achieved by executing leftward or rightward head movements during unidirectional rotation. , 1978, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[14]  F E Guedry,et al.  Visual counteraction on nauseogenic and disorienting effects of some whole-body motions: a proposed mechanism. , 1978, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[15]  Daniel Graupe,et al.  Identification of Systems , 1978, IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics.

[16]  M. Treisman Motion sickness: an evolutionary hypothesis. , 1977, Science.

[17]  Greg Zacharias Motion sensation dependence on visual and vestibular cues , 1977 .

[18]  E. Taub,et al.  Deafferentation in monkeys: Pointing at a target without visual feedback , 1975, Experimental Neurology.

[19]  Angular velocities, angular accelerations, and coriolis accelerations , 1975 .

[20]  S. S. Stevens Chapter 11 – PERCEPTUAL MAGNITUDE AND ITS MEASUREMENT , 1974 .

[21]  Laurence R. Young,et al.  Visual-vestibular interaction , 1994 .

[22]  J. Dichgans,et al.  Optokinetic motion sickness and pseudo-Coriolis effects induced by moving visual stimuli. , 1973, Acta oto-laryngologica.

[23]  Donald M. MacKay,et al.  Visual Stability and Voluntary Eye Movements , 1973 .

[24]  Huibert Kwakernaak,et al.  Linear Optimal Control Systems , 1972 .

[25]  A Graybiel,et al.  Direction-specific adaptation effects acquired in a slow rotation room. , 1972, Aerospace medicine.

[26]  A Graybiel,et al.  Response from arousal and thermal sweat areas during motion sickness. , 1972, Aerospace medicine.

[27]  D. McFarland Feedback mechanisms in animal behaviour , 1971 .

[28]  E. Evarts Central control of movement. V. Feedback and corollary discharge: a merging of the concepts. , 1971, Neurosciences Research Program bulletin.

[29]  David L. Kleinman,et al.  An optimal control model of human response part I: Theory and validation , 1970 .

[30]  A Graybiel,et al.  Changes in subjective estimates of well-being during the onset and remission of motion sickness symptomatology in the slow rotation room. , 1970, Aerospace medicine.

[31]  Fred E. Guedry,et al.  Conflicting sensory orientation cues as a factor in motion sickness , 1970 .

[32]  J. E. Steele The Symptomatology of Motion Sickness , 1970 .

[33]  A Graybiel,et al.  Structural elements in the concept of motion sickness. , 1969, Aerospace medicine.

[34]  David L. Kleinman,et al.  The Human as an Optimal Controller and Information Processor , 1969 .

[35]  James T. Reason,et al.  Motion sickness—some theoretical considerations , 1969 .

[36]  C. L. Thornton,et al.  Relationship between perceptual style and simulator sickness. , 1968, The Journal of applied psychology.

[37]  W. Wonham On the Separation Theorem of Stochastic Control , 1968 .

[38]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Diagnostic criteria for grading the severity of acute motion sickness. , 1968, Aerospace medicine.

[39]  A Graybiel,et al.  Transfer of habituation of motion sickness on change in body position between vertical and horizontal in a rotating environment. , 1968, Aerospace medicine.

[40]  Ashton Graybiel,et al.  Functional Disturbances of Vestibular Origin of Significance in Space Flight , 1967 .

[41]  F E Guedry,et al.  Habituation to Complex Vestibular Stimulation in Man: Transfer and Retention of Effects from Twelve Days of Rotation at 10 Rpm , 1965, Perceptual and motor skills.

[42]  E. Taub,et al.  ACQUISTION OF A TRACE-CONDITIONED AVOIDANCE RESPONSE AFTER DEAFFERENTATION OF THE RESPONDING LIMB. , 1965, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[43]  E. Sersen,et al.  Is Reafference Necessary for Visual Adaptation? , 1964, Perceptual and motor skills.

[44]  Richard Held,et al.  Motor-Sensory Feedback and the Geometry of Visual Space , 1963, Science.

[45]  R. Held,et al.  A Neural Model for Labile Sensorimotor Coordinations , 1962 .

[46]  J. E. Steele,et al.  Motion sickness and spatial perception. A theoretical study. , 1961, ASD technical report. United States. Air Force. Systems Command. Aeronautical Systems Division.

[47]  R. E. Kalman,et al.  New Results in Linear Filtering and Prediction Theory , 1961 .

[48]  R. Held,et al.  Neonatal deprivation and adult rearrangement: complementary techniques for analyzing plastic sensory-motor coordinations. , 1961, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[49]  R. Held Exposure-history as a factor in maintaining stability of perception and coordination. , 1961, The Journal of nervous and mental disease.

[50]  J. Groen Problems of the semicircular canal from a mechanico-physiological point of view. , 1961, Acta oto-laryngologica. Supplementum.

[51]  M. P. Lansberg A Primer of Space Medicine , 1960 .

[52]  R. E. Kalman,et al.  Contributions to the Theory of Optimal Control , 1960 .

[53]  S. C. Wang,et al.  Experimental motion sickness in dogs; importance of labyrinth and vestibular cerebellum. , 1956, The American journal of physiology.

[54]  G. H. Crampton,et al.  Studies of motion sickness. XVII. Physiological changes accompanying sickness in man. , 1955, Journal of applied physiology.

[55]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[56]  S. C. Wang,et al.  Experimental motion sickness in dogs; functional importance of chemoceptive emetic trigger zone. , 1954, The American journal of physiology.

[57]  K. Brizzee,et al.  A re‐evaluation of the cellular morphology of the area postrema in view of recent evidence for a chemoreceptor function , 1954, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[58]  H. L. Borison,et al.  A new concept of organization of the central emetic mechanism: recent studies on the sites of action of apomorphine, copper sulfate and cardiac glycosides. , 1952, Gastroenterology.

[59]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[60]  H. L. Borison,et al.  The vomiting center; a critical experimental analysis. , 1950, Archives of neurology and psychiatry.

[61]  H. A. Witkin Perception of body position and of the position of the visual field. , 1949 .

[62]  J. Needham Theoretische Biologie , 1933, Nature.

[63]  C. Beadnell THE PSYCHOLOGY OF SEA-SICKNESS. , 1924 .

[64]  C Marshbeadnell THE PSYCHOLOGY OF SEA-SICKNESS. , .

[65]  G. Stratton Vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1897 .

[66]  G. H. Oriel Pathology of Sea Sickness , 1889, The Hospital.

[67]  James William,et al.  SENSE OF DIZZINESS IN DEAF-MUTES , 1881 .