Premotor functional connectivity predicts impulsivity in juvenile offenders

Teenagers are often impulsive. In some cases this is a phase of normal development; in other cases impulsivity contributes to criminal behavior. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we examined resting-state functional connectivity among brain systems and behavioral measures of impulsivity in 107 juveniles incarcerated in a high-security facility. In less-impulsive juveniles and normal controls, motor planning regions were correlated with brain networks associated with spatial attention and executive control. In more-impulsive juveniles, these same regions correlated with the default-mode network, a constellation of brain areas associated with spontaneous, unconstrained, self-referential cognition. The strength of these brain–behavior relationships was sufficient to predict impulsivity scores at the individual level. Our data suggest that increased functional connectivity of motor-planning regions with networks subserving unconstrained, self-referential cognition, rather than those subserving executive control, heightens the predisposition to impulsive behavior in juvenile offenders. To further explore the relationship between impulsivity and neural development, we studied functional connectivity in the same motor-planning regions in 95 typically developing individuals across a wide age span. The change in functional connectivity with age mirrored that of impulsivity: younger subjects tended to exhibit functional connectivity similar to the more-impulsive incarcerated juveniles, whereas older subjects exhibited a less-impulsive pattern. This observation suggests that impulsivity in the offender population is a consequence of a delay in typical development, rather than a distinct abnormality.

[1]  A. Forth,et al.  The Hare Psychopathy Checklist—Youth Version. , 2005 .

[2]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Intrinsic Fluctuations within Cortical Systems Account for Intertrial Variability in Human Behavior , 2007, Neuron.

[3]  Anthony Randal McIntosh,et al.  Partial Least Squares (PLS) methods for neuroimaging: A tutorial and review , 2011, NeuroImage.

[4]  G. S. Pettit,et al.  Deficient Behavioral Inhibition and Anomalous Selective Attention in a Community Sample of Adolescents with Psychopathic Traits and Low-Anxiety Traits , 2005, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[5]  C. N. Macrae,et al.  Finding the Self? An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Primary and secondary saccades to goals defined by instructions , 1978, Vision Research.

[7]  Jonathan D. Power,et al.  Prediction of Individual Brain Maturity Using fMRI , 2010, Science.

[8]  R. Hare,et al.  Factor structure of the Psychopathy Checklist. , 1988, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.

[9]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited—Again , 1995, NeuroImage.

[10]  Edward T. Bullmore,et al.  Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks , 2010, NeuroImage.

[11]  Kerstin Konrad,et al.  Is the ADHD brain wired differently? A review on structural and functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder , 2010, Human brain mapping.

[12]  G L Shulman,et al.  INAUGURAL ARTICLE by a Recently Elected Academy Member:A default mode of brain function , 2001 .

[13]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Disruption of Large-Scale Brain Systems in Advanced Aging , 2007, Neuron.

[14]  Irwin M. Cohen,et al.  Predictive validity of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version for general and violent recidivism. , 2004, Behavioral sciences & the law.

[15]  R. Blair,et al.  Neuroimaging of Psychopathy and Antisocial Behavior: A Targeted Review , 2010, Current psychiatry reports.

[16]  J. Heckman,et al.  A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[17]  Troels W. Kjær,et al.  Reflective Self-Awareness and Conscious States: PET Evidence for a Common Midline Parietofrontal Core , 2002, NeuroImage.

[18]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Deanna M. Barch,et al.  Extracting core components of cognitive control , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  S. Petersen,et al.  Development of distinct control networks through segregation and integration , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  P. Fransson How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations , 2006, Neuropsychologia.

[22]  D. Kosson,et al.  Psychopathy and the allocation of attentional capacity in a divided-attention situation. , 1986, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[23]  Kaustubh Supekar,et al.  Development of Large-Scale Functional Brain Networks in Children , 2009, NeuroImage.

[24]  J. Arnett Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective , 1992 .

[25]  R. Blair,et al.  The development of antisocial behavior: What can we learn from functional neuroimaging studies? , 2008, Development and Psychopathology.

[26]  Vince D. Calhoun,et al.  A Hybrid Machine Learning Method for Fusing fMRI and Genetic Data: Combining both Improves Classification of Schizophrenia , 2010, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[27]  Robert Tibshirani,et al.  The Elements of Statistical Learning: Data Mining, Inference, and Prediction, 2nd Edition , 2001, Springer Series in Statistics.

[28]  Takashi Hanakawa,et al.  Functional coupling underlying motor and cognitive functions of the dorsal premotor cortex , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[29]  M. Raichle,et al.  Disease and the brain's dark energy , 2010, Nature Reviews Neurology.

[30]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Wandering Minds: The Default Network and Stimulus-Independent Thought , 2007, Science.

[31]  M. Honda,et al.  The role of rostral Brodmann area 6 in mental-operation tasks: an integrative neuroimaging approach. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[32]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  Maurizio Corbetta,et al.  The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[34]  M. Fukunaga,et al.  Low frequency BOLD fluctuations during resting wakefulness and light sleep: A simultaneous EEG‐fMRI study , 2008, Human brain mapping.

[35]  R. Dolan,et al.  Emotion, Cognition, and Behavior , 2002, Science.

[36]  M. Raichle,et al.  Effect of practice on reading performance and brain function , 2004, Neuroreport.

[37]  M. Fox,et al.  The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[38]  Richard Rogers,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Connectivity Changes during Decreasing Levels of Consciousness in Humans: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study using Propofol , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  Functional connectivity MRI in infants: Exploration of the functional organization of the developing brain , 2011, NeuroImage.

[40]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[41]  C. Odgers,et al.  The PCL: YV and recidivism in male and female juveniles: a follow-up into young adulthood. , 2008, International journal of law and psychiatry.

[42]  M. Boly,et al.  Breakdown of within- and between-network Resting State Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Connectivity during Propofol-induced Loss of Consciousness , 2010, Anesthesiology.

[43]  S. Petersen,et al.  The maturing architecture of the brain's default network , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  S. Petersen,et al.  Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[45]  D. Schacter,et al.  The Brain's Default Network , 2008, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[46]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Learning sculpts the spontaneous activity of the resting human brain , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[47]  M. Fox,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[48]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  E. Koechlin,et al.  The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition , 1999, Nature.

[50]  S. Petersen,et al.  A dual-networks architecture of top-down control , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[51]  Matthew R. G. Brown,et al.  Neural processes associated with antisaccade task performance investigated with event-related FMRI. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  W. Mischel,et al.  Delay of gratification in children. , 1989, Science.

[54]  R. Buckner,et al.  Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain's Default Network , 2010, Neuron.

[55]  M. Raichle,et al.  The Emotional Modulation of Cognitive Processing: An fMRI Study , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[56]  E. Crone,et al.  Neural evidence for dissociable components of task-switching. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[57]  Daniel L. Rubin,et al.  Network Analysis of Intrinsic Functional Brain Connectivity in Alzheimer's Disease , 2008, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[58]  A. Snyder,et al.  Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[59]  J. Duncan,et al.  Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands , 2000, Trends in Neurosciences.