Dogs Evaluate Threatening Facial Expressions by Their Biological Validity – Evidence from Gazing Patterns

Appropriate response to companions’ emotional signals is important for all social creatures. The emotional expressions of humans and non-human animals have analogies in their form and function, suggesting shared evolutionary roots, but very little is known about how animals other than primates view and process facial expressions. In primates, threat-related facial expressions evoke exceptional viewing patterns compared with neutral or positive stimuli. Here, we explore if domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) have such an attentional bias toward threatening social stimuli and whether observed emotional expressions affect dogs’ gaze fixation distribution among the facial features (eyes, midface and mouth). We recorded the voluntary eye gaze of 31 domestic dogs during viewing of facial photographs of humans and dogs with three emotional expressions (threatening, pleasant and neutral). We found that dogs’ gaze fixations spread systematically among facial features. The distribution of fixations was altered by the seen expression, but eyes were the most probable targets of the first fixations and gathered longer looking durations than mouth regardless of the viewed expression. The examination of the inner facial features as a whole revealed more pronounced scanning differences among expressions. This suggests that dogs do not base their perception of facial expressions on the viewing of single structures, but the interpretation of the composition formed by eyes, midface and mouth. Dogs evaluated social threat rapidly and this evaluation led to attentional bias, which was dependent on the depicted species: threatening conspecifics’ faces evoked heightened attention but threatening human faces instead an avoidance response. We propose that threatening signals carrying differential biological validity are processed via distinctive neurocognitive pathways. Both of these mechanisms may have an adaptive significance for domestic dogs. The findings provide a novel perspective on understanding the processing of emotional expressions and sensitivity to social threat in non-primates.

[1]  Masaki Tomonaga,et al.  Face scanning in chimpanzees and humans: continuity and discontinuity , 2010, Animal Behaviour.

[2]  C. Koch,et al.  A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention , 2000, Vision Research.

[3]  H. Aarts,et al.  Memory and attention for social threat: anxious hypercoding-avoidance and submissive gaze aversion. , 2012, Emotion.

[4]  Satoshi Hirata,et al.  Facial perception of conspecifics: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) preferentially attend to proper orientation and open eyes , 2010, Animal Cognition.

[5]  Thomas D. Albright,et al.  How Do Monkeys Look at Faces? , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Ádám Miklósi,et al.  What does it take to become ‘best friends’? Evolutionary changes in canine social competence , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Alex Martin,et al.  Threat, domain-specificity and the human amygdala , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  E. Blackwell,et al.  Human directed aggression in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris): Occurrence in different contexts and risk factors , 2014 .

[9]  L. Nummenmaa,et al.  Detection of emotional faces: salient physical features guide effective visual search. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[10]  Katherine M. Armstrong,et al.  Visuomotor Origins of Covert Spatial Attention , 2003, Neuron.

[11]  S. Marshall-Pescini,et al.  Dogs' Social Referencing towards Owners and Strangers , 2012, PloS one.

[12]  Matthew F. Peterson,et al.  Looking just below the eyes is optimal across face recognition tasks , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[13]  Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food? , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[14]  Kun Guo,et al.  Human Spontaneous Gaze Patterns in Viewing of Faces of Different Species , 2010, Perception.

[15]  Richard Dewhurst,et al.  Using eye-tracking to trace a cognitive process: Gaze behavior during decision making in a natural environment , 2013 .

[16]  J. Serpell,et al.  Breed differences in canine aggression , 2008 .

[17]  Márta Gácsi,et al.  A friend or an enemy? Dogs' reaction to an unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and friendliness at different times , 2005 .

[18]  Mara Mather,et al.  Beyond arousal and valence: The importance of the biological versus social relevance of emotional stimuli , 2012, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[19]  Katherine B. Martin,et al.  Facial Action Coding System , 2015 .

[20]  G. Cottrell,et al.  Two Fixations Suffice in Face Recognition , 2008, Psychological science.

[21]  B. Waller,et al.  Facial Expression in Nonhuman Animals , 2013 .

[22]  K. Mogg,et al.  A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. , 1998, Behaviour research and therapy.

[23]  Sasan Mahmoodi,et al.  How do monkeys view faces?—a study of eye movements , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Kun Guo,et al.  Holistic Gaze Strategy to Categorize Facial Expression of Varying Intensities , 2012, PloS one.

[25]  Thomas Armstrong,et al.  Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. , 2012, Clinical psychology review.

[26]  P. Ekman Emotion in the human face , 1982 .

[27]  Ludwig Huber,et al.  Dogs Can Discriminate Emotional Expressions of Human Faces , 2015, Current Biology.

[28]  M. Nagasawa,et al.  Dogs show left facial lateralization upon reunion with their owners , 2013, Behavioural Processes.

[29]  D. Custance,et al.  Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: an exploratory study , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[30]  Mélanie Perron,et al.  Featural processing in recognition of emotional facial expressions , 2014, Cognition & emotion.

[31]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Friend or foe: Context dependent sensitivity to human behaviour in dogs , 2010 .

[32]  Melissa J. Green,et al.  In the face of danger: Specific viewing strategies for facial expressions of threat? , 2003 .

[33]  D. Mills,et al.  Reading Faces: Differential Lateral Gaze Bias in Processing Canine and Human Facial Expressions in Dogs and 4-Year-Old Children , 2012, PloS one.

[34]  R. Ready,et al.  Attentional biases and memory for emotional stimuli in men and male rhesus monkeys , 2013, Animal Cognition.

[35]  A. J. Fridlund,et al.  Facial Expressions , 2018, Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science.

[36]  Christoph D. Dahl,et al.  Humans and Macaques Employ Similar Face-Processing Strategies , 2009, Current Biology.

[37]  P. Whalen,et al.  The structural and functional connectivity of the amygdala: From normal emotion to pathological anxiety , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[38]  G. Alpers,et al.  Happy mouth and sad eyes: scanning emotional facial expressions. , 2011, Emotion.

[39]  Sabine Hunnius,et al.  Facing threat: Infants' and adults' visual scanning of faces with neutral, happy, sad, angry, and fearful emotional expressions , 2011, Cognition & emotion.

[40]  E. Fox,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion: Are Angry Faces Detected More Efficiently? , 2000, Cognition & emotion.

[41]  Mark W. Becker,et al.  Please Scroll down for Article the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Early Detection and Avoidance of Threatening Faces during Passive Viewing , 2022 .

[42]  Juliane Kaminski,et al.  Do dogs get the point? A review of dog-human communication ability , 2013 .

[43]  W. Freiwald,et al.  The many facets of facial interactions in mammals , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[44]  F. Range,et al.  On the Way to a Better Understanding of Dog Domestication , 2014 .

[45]  Beatrice de Gelder,et al.  Emotional signals from faces, bodies and scenes influence observers' face expressions, fixations and pupil-size , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[46]  J. Call,et al.  Face and eye scanning in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), orangutans (Pongo abelii), and humans (Homo sapiens): unique eye-viewing patterns in humans among hominids. , 2012, Journal of comparative psychology.

[47]  H. Friedman,et al.  Classifying dogs’ (Canis familiaris) facial expressions from photographs , 2013, Behavioural Processes.

[48]  M. Tomonaga,et al.  Attention to emotional scenes including whole-body expressions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2010, Journal of comparative psychology.

[49]  A. Holmes,et al.  Evidence That Emotion Mediates Social Attention in Rhesus Macaques , 2012, PloS one.

[50]  M. Fox A Comparative Study of the Development of Facial Expressions in Canids; Wolf, Coyote and Foxes , 1970 .

[51]  Fiona J. Williams,et al.  Development of a head-mounted, eye-tracking system for dogs , 2011, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[52]  M. Nagasawa,et al.  Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions , 2011, Animal Cognition.

[53]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Angry faces hold the eyes , 2011 .

[54]  Juliane Kaminski,et al.  Paedomorphic Facial Expressions Give Dogs a Selective Advantage , 2013, PloS one.

[55]  K. Gothard,et al.  Videos of conspecifics elicit interactive looking patterns and facial expressions in monkeys. , 2011, Behavioral neuroscience.

[56]  Gang Chen,et al.  Distinct brain activity in processing negative pictures of animals and objects — The role of human contexts , 2014, NeuroImage.

[57]  S. G. Mazman,et al.  Examining the visual screening patterns of emotional facial expressions with gender, age and lateralization , 2013 .

[58]  C. Krause,et al.  How dogs scan familiar and inverted faces: an eye movement study , 2013, Animal Cognition.

[59]  Katalin M. Gothard,et al.  How do rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) scan faces in a visual paired comparison task? , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[60]  Heini Törnqvist,et al.  Dogs do look at images: eye tracking in canine cognition research , 2012, Animal Cognition.

[61]  S. Mineka,et al.  Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. , 2001, Psychological review.

[62]  C. Gray What Does It Take to Become a CFO , 2000 .

[63]  T. Penney,et al.  Humans Process Dog and Human Facial Affect in Similar Ways , 2013, PloS one.

[64]  C. Büchel,et al.  Diagnostic Features of Emotional Expressions Are Processed Preferentially , 2012, PLoS ONE.

[65]  Nirbhay N. Singh,et al.  Facial Expressions of Emotion , 1998 .

[66]  N. Bolwig Facial Expression in Primates With Remarks On a Parallel Development in Certain Carnivores (a Preliminary Report On Work in Progress) , 1964 .

[67]  K. Kendrick,et al.  Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence for face identity and face emotion processing in animals , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[68]  A. Lacreuse,et al.  Effects of testosterone on attention and memory for emotional stimuli in male rhesus monkeys , 2012, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[69]  C. Darwin The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , .

[70]  T. Brosch,et al.  The role of fear-relevant stimuli in visual search: a comparison of phylogenetic and ontogenetic stimuli. , 2005, Emotion.

[71]  B. Wood,et al.  On the origin, homologies and evolution of primate facial muscles, with a particular focus on hominoids and a suggested unifying nomenclature for the facial muscles of the Mammalia , 2009, Journal of anatomy.

[72]  Á. Miklósi,et al.  Dogs' Gaze Following Is Tuned to Human Communicative Signals , 2012, Current Biology.