Scientific Reasoning Across Different Domains

This study seeks to establish which scientific reasoning skills are primarily domain-general and which appear to be domain-specific. The subjects, 12 university undergraduates, each participated in self-directed experimentation with three different content domains. The experimentation contexts were computer-based laboratories in d.c. circuits (Voltaville), microeconomics (Smithtown), and the refraction of light (Refract), Subjects spent three 1-1/2 hour sessions working with each laboratory and took pretests and posttests that assessed their learning. Specific patterns of strategies used in each laboratory depended primarily on the structural form of the discovery task and the nature of the domain. In a situation that required the discovery of correlational regularities, evidence-generation activities, like the heuristic of controlling variables, were primary. In contexts where the regularities were functional rules, evidence interpretation became important. When the rules were quantitative, mathematical and algebraic heuristics were important. Students appeared very sensitive to the task demands of each laboratory, and adjusted their strategies accordingly. Regardless of this adaptation to specific conditions, they learned more as they proceeded from domain to domain, indicating that they were becoming more effective in planning and carrying out experiments, and in formulating and testing hypotheses based on those experiments. The findings suggest that the most generally useful skills for direct instruction may be those for evaluating the kind of problem at hand and for selecting the most appropriate processes and strategies.

[1]  Herbert A. Simon,et al.  The Processes of Scientific Discovery: The Strategy of Experimentation , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[2]  James Austin Laboratory life: The social construction of scientific facts: by Bruno Latour and Steve Woolgar. Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, 1979. , 1982 .

[3]  P. Wason On the Failure to Eliminate Hypotheses in a Conceptual Task , 1960 .

[4]  Marcia C. Linn,et al.  Is it formal if it's not physics? (the influence of content on formal reasoning) , 1983 .

[5]  H A SIMON,et al.  HUMAN ACQUISITION OF CONCEPTS FOR SEQUENTIAL PATTERNS. , 1963, Psychological review.

[6]  R. Sternberg,et al.  Learning and individual differences: Advances in theory and research. , 1989 .

[7]  S. Carey Cognitive science and science education. , 1986 .

[8]  J. E. Tschirgi,et al.  Sensible reasoning: A hypothesis about hypotheses. , 1980 .

[9]  Leona Schauble,et al.  Causal Models and Experimentation Strategies in Scientific Reasoning , 1991 .

[10]  J. Piaget,et al.  The Growth Of Logical Thinking From Childhood To Adolescence: An Essay On The Construction Of Formal Operational Structures , 1958 .

[11]  M. Chi,et al.  The Nature of Expertise , 1988 .

[12]  S. Carey Conceptual Change in Childhood , 1985 .

[13]  David Klahr,et al.  Dual Space Search During Scientific Reasoning , 1988, Cogn. Sci..

[14]  Ronald N. Giere,et al.  Understanding Scientific Reasoning , 1979 .

[15]  Valerie J. Shute,et al.  Inference and discovery in an exploratory laboratory. , 1988 .

[16]  Harriet Shaklee,et al.  Development of Rule Use in Judgments of Covariation between Events. , 1981 .

[17]  D. Kuhn Children and adults as intuitive scientists. , 1989, Psychological review.

[18]  M. Chi,et al.  Network representation of a child's dinosaur knowledge. , 1983 .