Mismatch negativity (MMN): an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep.

Sleep, unlike wakefulness, facilitates the internal stimulus generation and hinders the processing of external stimulation. Nevertheless, evidence yielded by physiological studies in animals and event-related potential (ERP) studies in humans suggest that basic functions of the central auditory system are still preserved during sleep. This review is focused on the automatic change-detection function of the auditory system as revealed by a negative ERP component called mismatch negativity (MMN). MMN mainly originates in the auditory cortex, although it also receives an important contribution from subcortical areas (especially at thalamic level), as well as frontal areas. We discuss recent experiments supporting the use of MMN as an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories not only during wakefulness, but also during sleep. The outcome of the activation of MMN generating system during sleep highly differs from that in waking, especially when there is no previous information about the stimulus sequence in the neuronal network as a result of learning. We discuss these differences in MMN generation in terms of a dynamicist view of the brain that emphasizes the importance of the integration between bottom-up and top-down influences on sensory processing, independently of the processing level in the auditory hierarchy.

[1]  Larry E. Roberts,et al.  Plastic changes in the auditory cortex induced by intensive frequency discrimination training , 2000, Neuroreport.

[2]  E. Schröger,et al.  Differential Contribution of Frontal and Temporal Cortices to Auditory Change Detection: fMRI and ERP Results , 2002, NeuroImage.

[3]  Mercedes Atienza,et al.  Complex sound processing during human REM sleep by recovering information from long-term memory as revealed by the mismatch negativity (MMN) , 2001, Brain Research.

[4]  Y. Niiyama,et al.  Endogenous components of event-related potential appearing during NREM stage 1 and REM sleep in man. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[5]  T. Carrell,et al.  Discrimination of speech-like contrasts in the auditory thalamus and cortex. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  K. Campbell,et al.  The mismatch negativity to frequency deviant stimuli during natural sleep. , 1996, Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology.

[7]  A. Braun,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep- wake cycle , 1997 .

[8]  L. Schiebinger,et al.  Commentary on Risto Naatanen (1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive fenctiono BBS 13s201-2888 , 1991 .

[9]  Carlos M. Gómez,et al.  The mismatch negativity component reveals the sensory memory during REM sleep in humans , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[10]  M Molnár,et al.  Evoked potential correlates of stimulus deviance during wakefulness and sleep in cat--animal model of mismatch negativity. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[11]  Naoko Shinozaki,et al.  The effect of deviant stimulus probability on the human mismatch process , 2000, Neuroreport.

[12]  R. Näätänen The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function , 1990, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[13]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Auditory frequency discrimination and event-related potentials. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[14]  R Hari,et al.  Deviant auditory stimuli activate human left and right auditory cortex differently. , 1996, Cerebral cortex.

[15]  F. Chollet,et al.  Differential fMRI Responses in the Left Posterior Superior Temporal Gyrus and Left Supramarginal Gyrus to Habituation and Change Detection in Syllables and Tones , 1999, NeuroImage.

[16]  J. L. Cantero,et al.  Decay time of the auditory sensory memory trace during wakefulness and REM sleep. , 2000, Psychophysiology.

[17]  M. Tervaniemi,et al.  Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. , 1993, Neuroreport.

[18]  Ulrich Schall,et al.  Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation during automatic auditory duration-mismatch processing in humans: a positron emission tomography study , 2001, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  I. Winkler,et al.  Presentation rate and magnitude of stimulus deviance effects on human pre-attentive change detection , 1995, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  P. Leppänen,et al.  Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool for investigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants and children , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[21]  I. Winkler,et al.  The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[22]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Processing of complex sounds in the human auditory cortex as revealed by magnetic brain responses. , 1996, Psychophysiology.

[23]  M. Sabri,et al.  Effects of sequential and temporal probability of deviant occurrence on mismatch negativity. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[24]  W. Singer,et al.  Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[25]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  H. Yabe,et al.  Automatic auditory information processing in sleep. , 2000, Sleep.

[27]  R Stickgold,et al.  Event‐related potentials (ERPs) to deviant auditory stimuli during sleep and waking , 1996, Neuroreport.

[28]  R. Näätänen Attention and brain function , 1992 .

[29]  J. Rohrbaugh,et al.  Current trends in event-related potential research , 1987 .

[30]  N. Cowan,et al.  The Role of Large-Scale Memory Organization in the Mismatch Negativity Event-Related Brain Potential , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  A. Kok,et al.  The mismatch negativity during natural sleep: intensity deviants , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[32]  H Lyytinen,et al.  Mismatch negativity during objective and subjective sleepiness. , 1997, Psychophysiology.

[33]  J. Martinerie,et al.  The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[34]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Sleep, Learning, and Dreams: Off-line Memory Reprocessing , 2001, Science.

[35]  V. Csépe,et al.  On the Origin and Development of the Mismatch Negativity , 1995, Ear and hearing.

[36]  A. A. Levin,et al.  The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Jean-Marc Edeline,et al.  Diversity of receptive field changes in auditory cortex during natural sleep , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[38]  N. Kraus,et al.  The time course of auditory perceptual learning: neurophysiological changes during speech‐sound training , 1998, Neuroreport.

[39]  H. Gould,et al.  Source Generators of Mismatch Negativity to Multiple Deviant Stimulus Types , 2004, Brain Topography.

[40]  R Cammann,et al.  Event-related potentials to pitch change in an auditory stimulus sequence during sleep. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Supplement.

[41]  I. Winkler,et al.  Mismatch negativity is unaffected by top-down predictive information , 2001, Neuroreport.

[42]  J. Armony,et al.  Auditory Processing across the Sleep-Wake Cycle Simultaneous EEG and fMRI Monitoring in Humans , 2000, Neuron.

[43]  I. Winkler,et al.  The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitive neuroscience. , 1999, Psychological bulletin.

[44]  P Maquet,et al.  The Role of Sleep in Learning and Memory , 2001, Science.

[45]  A. Muller-Gass,et al.  Event-related potential measures of the inhibition of information processing: I. Selective attention in the waking state. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[46]  H Lyytinen,et al.  Processing of auditory stimuli during tonic and phasic periods of REM sleep as revealed by event‐related brain potentials , 1996, Journal of sleep research.

[47]  Y Kuroiwa,et al.  [Evoked potential]. , 1999, Rinsho shinkeigaku = Clinical neurology.

[48]  J. L. Cantero,et al.  The time course of neural changes underlying auditory perceptual learning. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[49]  R. Stickgold,et al.  Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[50]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Superior formation of cortical memory traces for melodic patterns in musicians. , 2001, Learning & memory.

[51]  L. Deouell,et al.  Mismatch negativity in dichotic listening: evidence for interhemispheric differences and multiple generators. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[52]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Psychobiology: Speech sounds learned by sleeping newborns , 2002, Nature.

[53]  Neuronal plasticity induced by fear conditioning is expressed during paradoxical sleep: evidence from simultaneous recordings in the lateral amygdala and the medial geniculate in rats. , 1998, Behavioral neuroscience.

[54]  H Lyytinen,et al.  Is the appearance of mismatch negativity during stage 2 sleep related to the elicitation of K-complex? , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[55]  C. Degueldre,et al.  Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming , 1996, Nature.

[56]  J. Kenemans,et al.  Auditory event-related potentials to deviant stimuli during drowsiness and stage 2 sleep. , 1995, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[57]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magnetic brain responses , 1997, Nature.

[58]  Nina Kraus,et al.  Acoustic features and acoustic change are represented by different central pathways , 1995, Hearing Research.

[59]  R. Knight,et al.  A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans , 1998, Brain Research.

[60]  D L Woods,et al.  Differential auditory processing continues during sleep. , 1991, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[61]  C. Escera,et al.  Auditory information processing during human sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials , 2001, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[62]  N. Kraus,et al.  Nonprimary auditory thalamic representation of acoustic change. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[63]  T. Carrell,et al.  Central Auditory System Plasticity Associated with Speech Discrimination Training , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  A. Braun,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2(15)O PET study. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[65]  A. Coenen,et al.  Animal models for information processing during sleep. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[66]  K. Alho,et al.  Separate Time Behaviors of the Temporal and Frontal Mismatch Negativity Sources , 2000, NeuroImage.

[67]  R. Ilmoniemi,et al.  Processing of novel sounds and frequency changes in the human auditory cortex: magnetoencephalographic recordings. , 1998, Psychophysiology.

[68]  O Bertrand,et al.  Analysis of speech sounds is left-hemisphere predominant at 100-150ms after sound onset. , 1999, Neuroreport.

[69]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Human auditory-cortex mechanisms of preattentive sound discrimination , 2000, Neuroscience Letters.

[70]  R. Näätänen,et al.  Attention and mismatch negativity. , 1993, Psychophysiology.

[71]  G. Karmos,et al.  Adaptive modeling of the unattended acoustic environment reflected in the mismatch negativity event-related potential , 1996, Brain Research.