An iterative probabilistic model of speech-related vocalization rate growth due to child-caregiver interaction
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Umit Yapanel,et al. Reliability of the LENA Language Environment Analysis System in Young Children’s Natural Home Environment , 2009 .
[2] A. Wetherby,et al. Communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: implications for early identification. , 1989, The Journal of speech and hearing disorders.
[3] G. Westermann,et al. A new model of sensorimotor coupling in the development of speech , 2004, Brain and Language.
[4] A. Karmiloff-Smith. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[5] G. Dawson,et al. Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[6] Pierre-Yves Oudeyer,et al. Self-organization of early vocal development in infants and machines: the role of intrinsic motivation , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[7] A. Neal,et al. Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism* , 2000 .
[8] D K Oller,et al. Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[9] Michael Ramscar,et al. Children Value Informativity Over Logic in Word Learning , 2013, Psychological science.
[10] F. Tion,et al. Reliability of the LENA TM Language Environment Analysis System in Young Children's Natural Home Environment , 2009 .
[11] I. Howard,et al. Modeling the development of pronunciation in infant speech acquisition. , 2011, Motor control.
[12] Melody Dye,et al. The Effects of Feature-Label-Order and Their Implications for Symbolic Learning , 2010, Cogn. Sci..
[13] Tetsuya Ogata,et al. Continuous vocal imitation with self-organized vowel spaces in Recurrent Neural Network , 2009, 2009 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation.
[14] Katharina J. Rohlfing,et al. Microdynamics of Interaction: Capturing and Modeling Infants' Social Learning [Guest Editorial] , 2013, IEEE Trans. Auton. Ment. Dev..
[15] M. Goldstein,et al. Social interaction shapes babbling: Testing parallels between birdsong and speech , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[16] Elena Patten,et al. Vocal Patterns in Infants with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Canonical Babbling Status and Vocalization Frequency , 2014, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
[17] Andrew Pickles,et al. Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. , 2007, Journal of consulting and clinical psychology.
[18] Satrajit S. Ghosh,et al. Neural modeling and imaging of the cortical interactions underlying syllable production , 2006, Brain and Language.
[19] Bernd J. Kröger,et al. Towards a neurocomputational model of speech production and perception , 2009, Speech Commun..
[20] Anne Fernald,et al. Talking to Children Matters , 2013, Psychological science.
[21] John H. L. Hansen,et al. Signal processing for young child speech language development , 2008, WOCCI.
[22] E. Hoff. The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. , 2003, Child development.
[23] Erika Hoff,et al. Socioeconomic Status and Parenting , 2019, Handbook of Parenting.
[24] Ping Li,et al. Early lexical development in a self-organizing neural network , 2004, Neural Networks.
[25] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[26] Chrystopher L. Nehaniv,et al. Interactive Language Learning by Robots: The Transition from Babbling to Word Forms , 2012, PloS one.
[27] Sue Peppé,et al. Receptive and expressive prosodic ability in children with high-functioning autism. , 2007, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.
[28] J. Iverson,et al. Maternal verbal responses to communication of infants at low and heightened risk of autism , 2014, Autism : the international journal of research and practice.
[29] R. Rescorla. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .
[30] P. Mundy,et al. Joint Attention and Early Social Communication: Implications for Research on Intervention with Autism , 1997, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[31] Marilyn M. Vihman,et al. Supporting Early Vocabulary Development: What Sort of Responsiveness Matters? , 2013, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.
[32] S. Suter. Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children , 2005, European Journal of Pediatrics.
[33] Gordon Ramsay,et al. Out of the mouths of babes: vocal production in infant siblings of children with ASD. , 2011, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[34] Anne S. Warlaumont,et al. Salience-based reinforcement of a spiking neural network leads to increased syllable production , 2013, 2013 IEEE Third Joint International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL).
[35] Minoru Asada,et al. Vowel Acquisition Based on an Auto-Mirroring Bias with a Less Imitative Caregiver , 2012, Adv. Robotics.
[36] Jill Gilkerson,et al. A Social Feedback Loop for Speech Development and Its Reduction in Autism , 2014, Psychological science.
[37] Ami Klin,et al. Young autistic children's listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal , 1991, Journal of autism and developmental disorders.
[38] Anne Fernald,et al. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. , 2013, Developmental science.
[39] Gert Westermann,et al. Prespeech motor learning in a neural network using reinforcement , 2013, Neural Networks.