Position of code and code for position: From isomorphism to a sensorimotor account of space perception
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] B. Bridgeman,et al. Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[2] D. C. Higgins. Human Spatial Orientation , 1967, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[3] Donald M. MacKay,et al. Visual Stability and Voluntary Eye Movements , 1973 .
[4] J R Duhamel,et al. The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.
[5] A. Noë,et al. A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[6] W. Prinz. Ideomotorik und Isomorphie , 1985 .
[7] P. Wolff. Saccadic Eye Movements and Visual Stability: Preliminary Considerations Towards a Cognitive Approach , 1984 .
[8] H. Honda. The time courses of visual mislocalization and of extraretinal eye position signals at the time of vertical saccades , 1991, Vision Research.
[9] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. Action influences spatial perception: Neuropsychological evidence , 2004 .
[10] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. Chapter 1 Separate representations of visual space for perception and visually guided behavior , 1999 .
[11] A. Allport,et al. Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .
[12] G. Roth,et al. Evolution of the brain and intelligence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[13] B Bridgeman,et al. Multiple sources of outflow in processing spatial information. , 1986, Acta psychologica.
[14] C. Gray. The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration Still Alive and Well , 1999, Neuron.
[15] L. Kempe. Handbook of Physiology. Section I. The Nervous System , 1982 .
[16] J. Müsseler,et al. Perceived localizations and eye movements with action‐generated and computer‐generated vanishing points of moving stimuli , 2004 .
[17] R. Welch. Perceptual Modification: Adapting to Altered Sensory Environments , 1978 .
[18] A. Treisman. The binding problem , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[19] C. Blakemore,et al. The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.
[20] R. Sperry. Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[21] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[22] O. D. Creutzfeldt,et al. Representation of complex visual stimuli in the brain , 1978, Naturwissenschaften.
[23] Wolfram Erlhagen,et al. The role of action plans and other cognitive factors in motion extrapolation: A modelling study , 2004 .
[24] P. Cz.. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 1896 .
[25] L. Festinger,et al. Efference and the conscious experience of perception , 1967 .
[26] P. Wolff. Wahrnehmungslernen durch Blickbewegungen , 1985 .
[27] J. Gibson,et al. Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. , 1933 .
[28] Bruce Bridgeman,et al. A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[29] Christoph von der Malsburg,et al. The Correlation Theory of Brain Function , 1994 .
[30] P. Wolff. Saccadic exploration and perceptual-motor learning. , 1986, Acta psychologica.
[31] D. Dennett,et al. The Nature of Consciousness , 2006 .
[32] E. Brenner,et al. Stability relative to what? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[33] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] E. Scheerer. The constitution of space perception: a phenomenological perspective. , 1986, Acta psychologica.
[35] James J. Clark,et al. Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.
[36] S. Coren,et al. Eye movements and decrement in the Oppel-Kundt illusion , 1972 .
[37] R S Slotnick,et al. Adaptation to curvature distortion. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.
[38] A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al. Sources of position-perception error for small isolated targets , 1999, Psychological research.
[39] R. W. Ditchburn. Seeing is Deceiving: The Psychology of Visual Illusions , 1979 .
[40] Christoph von der Malsburg,et al. The What and Why of Binding The Modeler’s Perspective , 1999, Neuron.
[41] Stanley Coren,et al. An alternative view of the “Gibson normalization effect” , 1967 .
[42] Jochen Müsseler,et al. How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally ranked crosstalks , 1999 .
[43] H W Leibowitz,et al. Efference, perceived movement, and illusory displacement. , 1986, Acta psychologica.
[44] W. Köhler. Gestalt psychology , 1967 .
[45] O. Neumann. Visual Attention and Action , 1990 .
[46] Nicola Smania,et al. Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients , 2001, NeuroImage.
[47] Function and processing of “meaningless” and “meaningful” position: Commentary on Van der Heijden et al. , 1999 .
[48] Leonard Matin,et al. Visual Localization and Eye Movements , 1982 .
[49] Glyn W. Humphreys Dietmar Heinke,et al. Spatial Representation and Selection in the Brain: Neuropsychological and Computational Constraints , 1998 .
[50] Ralf Engbert,et al. Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.
[51] R. Day. The effects of repeated trials and prolonged fixation on error in the Müller-Lyer figure. , 1962 .
[52] C. A. Burnham. Decrement of the Müller-Lyer illusion with saccadic and tracking eye movements , 1968 .
[53] B. Bridgeman,et al. Relations between the physiology of attention and the physiology of consciousness , 1986, Psychological research.
[54] E. Marg. A VISION OF THE BRAIN , 1994 .
[55] O. Creutzfeldt. Neurophysiological mechanisms and consciousness. , 1979, Ciba Foundation symposium.
[56] D. Mackay. Evaluation: The Missing Link Between Cognition and Action , 1984 .
[57] W Singer,et al. Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.
[58] Helen Pritchard,et al. The effect of practice on the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion. , 2022 .
[59] O. Neumann. Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .
[60] Jochen Müsseler,et al. Two spatial maps for perceived visual space: Evidence from relative mislocalizations , 2004 .
[61] W. Shebilske. Extraretinal information in corrective saccades and inflow vs outflow theories of visual direction constancy , 1976, Vision Research.
[62] G. Brindley,et al. The absence of position sense in the human eye , 1960, The Journal of physiology.
[63] D. Hubel,et al. Stereoscopic Vision in Macaque Monkey: Cells sensitive to Binocular Depth in Area 18 of the Macaque Monkey Cortex , 1970, Nature.
[64] Eckart Scheerer,et al. Psychoneural isomorphism: Historical background and current relevance , 1994 .
[65] Robert M. Steinman,et al. The extraretinal signal for the visual perception of direction , 1972 .
[66] David A. Atchison,et al. Optics of the Human Eye , 2023 .
[67] G. Luppino,et al. Parietofrontal Circuits for Action and Space Perception in the Macaque Monkey , 2001, NeuroImage.