Position of code and code for position: From isomorphism to a sensorimotor account of space perception

The paper starts with a discussion of the assumption that positions of the outer world are coded by the anatomical locations of firing neurons within retinotopic maps (“coding position by position”). This “code position theory” explains space perception by some kind of structural isomorphism since it implies that the perceptual space is based on a spatial structure within the brain. The axiom of structural isomorphism is rejected. Subsequently, a sensorimotor account of space perception is outlined according to which the spatial structure of the outer world is coded by the temporal structure of cortical processing. The basis is that action changes the perceiver's relationship to the outer world and, therefore, changes the representation of the outer world coded by the sensory responses of the brain. According to this view the code for position is not a spatial but a temporal structure resulting from action (“coding position by action”). The sensorimotor account offers a possible solution to the binding problem. The paper ends with some remarks on the possible origin and function of retinotopic representations.

[1]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relation between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  D. C. Higgins Human Spatial Orientation , 1967, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.

[3]  Donald M. MacKay,et al.  Visual Stability and Voluntary Eye Movements , 1973 .

[4]  J R Duhamel,et al.  The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. , 1992, Science.

[5]  A. Noë,et al.  A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[6]  W. Prinz Ideomotorik und Isomorphie , 1985 .

[7]  P. Wolff Saccadic Eye Movements and Visual Stability: Preliminary Considerations Towards a Cognitive Approach , 1984 .

[8]  H. Honda The time courses of visual mislocalization and of extraretinal eye position signals at the time of vertical saccades , 1991, Vision Research.

[9]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Action influences spatial perception: Neuropsychological evidence , 2004 .

[10]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  Chapter 1 Separate representations of visual space for perception and visually guided behavior , 1999 .

[11]  A. Allport,et al.  Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action , 1987 .

[12]  G. Roth,et al.  Evolution of the brain and intelligence , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[13]  B Bridgeman,et al.  Multiple sources of outflow in processing spatial information. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[14]  C. Gray The Temporal Correlation Hypothesis of Visual Feature Integration Still Alive and Well , 1999, Neuron.

[15]  L. Kempe Handbook of Physiology. Section I. The Nervous System , 1982 .

[16]  J. Müsseler,et al.  Perceived localizations and eye movements with action‐generated and computer‐generated vanishing points of moving stimuli , 2004 .

[17]  R. Welch Perceptual Modification: Adapting to Altered Sensory Environments , 1978 .

[18]  A. Treisman The binding problem , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[19]  C. Blakemore,et al.  The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  R. Sperry Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. , 1950, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[21]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[22]  O. D. Creutzfeldt,et al.  Representation of complex visual stimuli in the brain , 1978, Naturwissenschaften.

[23]  Wolfram Erlhagen,et al.  The role of action plans and other cognitive factors in motion extrapolation: A modelling study , 2004 .

[24]  P. Cz. Handbuch der physiologischen Optik , 1896 .

[25]  L. Festinger,et al.  Efference and the conscious experience of perception , 1967 .

[26]  P. Wolff Wahrnehmungslernen durch Blickbewegungen , 1985 .

[27]  J. Gibson,et al.  Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. , 1933 .

[28]  Bruce Bridgeman,et al.  A theory of visual stability across saccadic eye movements , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[29]  Christoph von der Malsburg,et al.  The Correlation Theory of Brain Function , 1994 .

[30]  P. Wolff Saccadic exploration and perceptual-motor learning. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[31]  D. Dennett,et al.  The Nature of Consciousness , 2006 .

[32]  E. Brenner,et al.  Stability relative to what? , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[33]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[34]  E. Scheerer The constitution of space perception: a phenomenological perspective. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[35]  James J. Clark,et al.  Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts , 2002, Vision Research.

[36]  S. Coren,et al.  Eye movements and decrement in the Oppel-Kundt illusion , 1972 .

[37]  R S Slotnick,et al.  Adaptation to curvature distortion. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[38]  A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al.  Sources of position-perception error for small isolated targets , 1999, Psychological research.

[39]  R. W. Ditchburn Seeing is Deceiving: The Psychology of Visual Illusions , 1979 .

[40]  Christoph von der Malsburg,et al.  The What and Why of Binding The Modeler’s Perspective , 1999, Neuron.

[41]  Stanley Coren,et al.  An alternative view of the “Gibson normalization effect” , 1967 .

[42]  Jochen Müsseler,et al.  How independent from action control is perception? An event-coding account for more equally ranked crosstalks , 1999 .

[43]  H W Leibowitz,et al.  Efference, perceived movement, and illusory displacement. , 1986, Acta psychologica.

[44]  W. Köhler Gestalt psychology , 1967 .

[45]  O. Neumann Visual Attention and Action , 1990 .

[46]  Nicola Smania,et al.  Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients , 2001, NeuroImage.

[47]  Function and processing of “meaningless” and “meaningful” position: Commentary on Van der Heijden et al. , 1999 .

[48]  Leonard Matin,et al.  Visual Localization and Eye Movements , 1982 .

[49]  Glyn W. Humphreys Dietmar Heinke,et al.  Spatial Representation and Selection in the Brain: Neuropsychological and Computational Constraints , 1998 .

[50]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention , 2003, Vision Research.

[51]  R. Day The effects of repeated trials and prolonged fixation on error in the Müller-Lyer figure. , 1962 .

[52]  C. A. Burnham Decrement of the Müller-Lyer illusion with saccadic and tracking eye movements , 1968 .

[53]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Relations between the physiology of attention and the physiology of consciousness , 1986, Psychological research.

[54]  E. Marg A VISION OF THE BRAIN , 1994 .

[55]  O. Creutzfeldt Neurophysiological mechanisms and consciousness. , 1979, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[56]  D. Mackay Evaluation: The Missing Link Between Cognition and Action , 1984 .

[57]  W Singer,et al.  Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[58]  Helen Pritchard,et al.  The effect of practice on the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion. , 2022 .

[59]  O. Neumann Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention , 1987 .

[60]  Jochen Müsseler,et al.  Two spatial maps for perceived visual space: Evidence from relative mislocalizations , 2004 .

[61]  W. Shebilske Extraretinal information in corrective saccades and inflow vs outflow theories of visual direction constancy , 1976, Vision Research.

[62]  G. Brindley,et al.  The absence of position sense in the human eye , 1960, The Journal of physiology.

[63]  D. Hubel,et al.  Stereoscopic Vision in Macaque Monkey: Cells sensitive to Binocular Depth in Area 18 of the Macaque Monkey Cortex , 1970, Nature.

[64]  Eckart Scheerer,et al.  Psychoneural isomorphism: Historical background and current relevance , 1994 .

[65]  Robert M. Steinman,et al.  The extraretinal signal for the visual perception of direction , 1972 .

[66]  David A. Atchison,et al.  Optics of the Human Eye , 2023 .

[67]  G. Luppino,et al.  Parietofrontal Circuits for Action and Space Perception in the Macaque Monkey , 2001, NeuroImage.