Visual object recognition.

Visual object recognition is of fundamental importance to most animals. The diversity of tasks that any biological recognition system must solve suggests that object recognition is not a single, general purpose process. In this review, we consider evidence from the fields of psychology, neuropsychology, and neurophysiology, all of which supports the idea that there are multiple systems for recognition. Data from normal adults, infants, animals, and brain damaged patients reveal a major distinction between the classification of objects at a basic category level and the identification of individual objects from a homogeneous object class. An additional distinction between object representations used for visual perception and those used for visually guided movements provides further support for a multiplicity of visual recognition systems. Recent evidence from psychophysical and neurophysiological studies indicates that one system may represent objects by combinations of multiple views, or aspects, and another may represent objects by structural primitives and their spatial interrelationships.

[1]  D. MacRae,et al.  The defect of function in visual agnosia. , 1956, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[2]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. , 1956, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  R. Brown How shall a thing be called. , 1958, Psychological review.

[4]  R. L. Fantz Visual Experience in Infants: Decreased Attention to Familiar Patterns Relative to Novel Ones , 1964, Science.

[5]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Complex Visual Concept in the Pigeon , 1964, Science.

[6]  M. Critchley The parietal lobes , 1966 .

[7]  M. Posner,et al.  Perceived distance and the classification of distorted patterns. , 1967, Journal of experimental psychology.

[8]  E. Renzi,et al.  The Performance of Patients with Unilateral Brain Damage on Face Recognition Tasks , 1968 .

[9]  M. Posner,et al.  On the genesis of abstract ideas. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[10]  H. Munitz,et al.  Prosopagnosia with animal face agnosia. , 1969, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[11]  M Mishkin,et al.  Further evidence on the locus of the visual area in the temporal lobe of the monkey. , 1969, Experimental neurology.

[12]  N. S. Sutherland,et al.  Shape discrimination by goldfish: Coding of irregularities. , 1970 .

[13]  J. Jenkins,et al.  Abstraction over time of prototypes from distortions of random dot patterns: A replication , 1970 .

[14]  J. Bransford,et al.  Abstraction of visual patterns. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[15]  Stephen K. Reed,et al.  Pattern recognition and categorization , 1972 .

[16]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Innate and conditioned reactions to threat in rats with amygdaloid lesions. , 1972, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[18]  D. Homa Prototype abstraction and classification of new instances as a function of number of instances defining the prototype , 1973 .

[19]  A. Taylor,et al.  The contribution of the right parietal lobe to object recognition. , 1973, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[20]  D. Shinar,et al.  Effects of form rotation on the speed of classification: the development of shape constancy , 1973 .

[21]  Dave Bartram,et al.  The role of visual and semantic codes in object naming , 1974 .

[22]  C. C. Goren,et al.  Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. , 1975, Pediatrics.

[23]  Daniel F. Chambliss,et al.  The relative contributions of common and distinctive information on the abstraction from ill-defined categories , 1975 .

[24]  D. Bartram Levels of coding in picture-picture comparison tasks , 1976, Memory & cognition.

[25]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  R. Herrnstein,et al.  Natural concepts in pigeons. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[27]  G. V. Hoesen,et al.  Temporal neocortical afferent connections to the amygdala in the rhesus monkey , 1976, Brain Research.

[28]  E. Rosch,et al.  Structural bases of typicality effects. , 1976 .

[29]  D. Homa,et al.  Category Breadth and the Abstraction of Prototypical Information. , 1976 .

[30]  Stephen A. Ritz,et al.  Distinctive features, categorical perception, and probability learning: some applications of a neural model , 1977 .

[31]  E. Warrington,et al.  Prosopagnosia: a clinical, psychological, and anatomical study of three patients. , 1977, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[32]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Contributions of the corpus callosum and the anterior commissure to visual activation of inferior temporal neurons , 1977, Brain Research.

[33]  S. Palmer Hierarchical structure in perceptual representation , 1977, Cognitive Psychology.

[34]  M. Corballis,et al.  Decisions about identity and orientation of rotated letters and digits , 1978, Memory & cognition.

[35]  D. Robinson,et al.  Parietal association cortex in the primate: sensory mechanisms and behavioral modulations. , 1978, Journal of neurophysiology.

[36]  D. Pandya,et al.  Afferent cortical connections and architectonics of the superior temporal sulcus and surrounding cortex in the rhesus monkey , 1978, Brain Research.

[37]  E. Yeterian,et al.  Cortico-striate projections in the rhesus monkey: The organization of certain cortico-caudate connections , 1978, Brain Research.

[38]  D. Marr,et al.  Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes , 1978, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[39]  J. Cerella Visual classes and natural categories in the pigeon. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  A. Damasio,et al.  Impairment of hand movements under visual guidance , 1979, Neurology.

[41]  G. V. Hoesen,et al.  Face recognition in the rhesus monkey , 1979, Neuropsychologia.

[42]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque , 1979, Brain Research.

[43]  R. Desimone,et al.  Prestriate afferents to inferior temporal cortex: an HRP study , 1980, Brain Research.

[44]  M Mishkin,et al.  Organization of the amygdalopetal projections from modality‐specific cortical association areas in the monkey , 1980, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[45]  A. Mikami,et al.  Inferotemporal neuron activities and color discrimination with delay , 1980, Brain Research.

[46]  Visual mechanisms in the temporal and prestriate association cortices of the monkey , 1981 .

[47]  D. Homa,et al.  Limitations of exemplar-based generalization and the abstraction of categorical information. , 1981 .

[48]  I. Rock,et al.  The effect on form perception of change of orientation in the third dimension. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  H. Barbas,et al.  Organization of afferent input to subdivisions of area 8 in the rhesus monkey , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[50]  F Simion,et al.  Transformation processes upon the visual code , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[51]  C. Bruce,et al.  Face recognition by monkeys: Absence of an inversion effect , 1982, Neuropsychologia.

[52]  G. V. Van Hoesen,et al.  Prosopagnosia , 1982, Neurology.

[53]  M. G. Eley,et al.  Identifying rotated letter-like symbols , 1982, Memory & cognition.

[54]  G. V. Hoesen,et al.  The parahippocampal gyrus: New observations regarding its cortical connections in the monkey , 1982, Trends in Neurosciences.

[55]  R. Desimone,et al.  Shape recognition and inferior temporal neurons. , 1983, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  The nature and structure of infant form categories , 1983 .

[57]  J.A. Anderson,et al.  Theory of categorization based on distributed memory storage. , 1984 .

[58]  B. Tversky,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : General VOL . 113 , No . 2 JUNE 1984 Objects , Parts , and Categories , 2005 .

[59]  M. Corballis,et al.  Winding one's ps and qs: mental rotation and mirror-image discrimination. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[61]  Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al.  Pictures and names: Making the connection , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[62]  E. Rolls Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. , 1984, Human neurobiology.

[63]  V. Mountcastle The visual functions of the parietal lobe , 1984, Behavioural Brain Research.

[64]  Rolls Et Neurons in the cortex of the temporal lobe and in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. , 1984 .

[65]  E. Iwai Neuropsychological basis of pattern vision in macaque monkeys , 1985, Vision Research.

[66]  P. Jolicoeur The time to name disoriented natural objects , 1985, Memory & cognition.

[67]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces , 1985, Behavioural Brain Research.

[68]  A. J. Mistlin,et al.  Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction , 1985, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[69]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[70]  H. Brownell,et al.  Category differentiation in object recognition: typicality constraints on the basic category advantage. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[71]  Paul C. Quinn,et al.  On Categorization in Early Infancy. , 1986 .

[72]  Slowly Progressive Visual Agnosia or Apraxia Without Dementia , 1986, Cortex.

[73]  E. De Renzi Slowly progressive visual agnosia or apraxia without dementia. , 1986, Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior.

[74]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Projections to the superior temporal sulcus from the central and peripheral field representations of V1 and V2 , 1986, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[75]  S. Carey,et al.  Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[76]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in area V4 of the macaque: sensitivity to stimulus form. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[77]  J. P. Cavanagh,et al.  Reconstructing the third dimension: Interactions between color, texture, motion, binocular disparity, and shape , 1987, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[78]  T Shiwa,et al.  Corticocortical projections to the monkey temporal lobe with particular reference to the visual processing pathways. , 1987, Archives italiennes de biologie.

[79]  E. Rolls,et al.  Functional subdivisions of the temporal lobe neocortex , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[80]  H. Spitzer,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. I. Response characteristics. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[81]  I. Biederman Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.

[82]  P. Jolicoeur A size-congruency effect in memory for visual shape , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[83]  A. Young,et al.  Configurational Information in Face Perception , 1987, Perception.

[84]  I. Rock,et al.  A case of viewer-centered object perception , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[85]  M. Mishkin,et al.  The anatomy of memory. , 1987, Scientific American.

[86]  V S Ramachandran,et al.  Perceiving shape from shading. , 1988, Scientific American.

[87]  A. Damasio,et al.  Intact recognition of facial expression, gender, and age in patients with impaired recognition of face identity , 1988, Neurology.

[88]  T. Valentine Upside-down faces: a review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. , 1988, British journal of psychology.

[89]  H. Barbas Anatomic organization of basoventral and mediodorsal visual recipient prefrontal regions in the rhesus monkey , 1988, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[90]  S. Ullman Aligning pictorial descriptions: An approach to object recognition , 1989, Cognition.

[91]  A. Milner,et al.  A Disorder of Lightness Discrimination in a Case of Visual Form Agnosia , 1989, Cortex.

[92]  R. Andersen Visual and eye movement functions of the posterior parietal cortex. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[93]  D I Perrett,et al.  Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[94]  M. Tarr,et al.  Mental rotation and orientation-dependence in shape recognition , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[95]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Varieties of Object Constancy , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[96]  I. Rock,et al.  Can we imagine how objects look from other viewpoints? , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.

[97]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Some results on translation invariance in the human visual system. , 1990, Spatial vision.

[98]  E Tulving,et al.  Priming and human memory systems. , 1990, Science.

[99]  A. Damasio,et al.  Face agnosia and the neural substrates of memory. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[100]  T. Poggio A theory of how the brain might work. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[101]  Shigeru Watanabe,et al.  Discrimination of Individuals in Pigeons , 1990 .

[102]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Visual topography of area TEO in the macaque , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[103]  Belur V. Dasarathy,et al.  Nearest neighbor (NN) norms: NN pattern classification techniques , 1991 .

[104]  J. Cronly-Dillon,et al.  Visual Agnosias and Other Disturbances of Visual Perception and Cognition , 1991 .

[105]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[106]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. , 1991, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[107]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[108]  M. Turk,et al.  Eigenfaces for Recognition , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[109]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline , 1991, Cognition.

[110]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.

[111]  Minami Ito,et al.  Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 1992, Nature.

[112]  E T Rolls,et al.  Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[113]  D I Perrett,et al.  Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[114]  P. Jolicoeur,et al.  Reference frame and effects of orientation on finding the tops of rotated objects. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[115]  G. Humphrey,et al.  Recognizing novel views of three-dimensional objects. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[116]  A. Cowey,et al.  The role of the 'face-cell' area in the discrimination and recognition of faces by monkeys. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[117]  I. Biederman,et al.  Size invariance in visual object priming , 1992 .

[118]  D L Schacter,et al.  Priming and recognition of transformed three-dimensional objects: effects of size and reflection. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[119]  P Jolicoeur,et al.  Size effects in visual recognition memory are determined by perceived size , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[120]  Michael Davis,et al.  The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. , 1992, Annual review of neuroscience.

[121]  H H Bülthoff,et al.  Psychophysical support for a two-dimensional view interpolation theory of object recognition. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[122]  J. O'Regan,et al.  Solving the "real" mysteries of visual perception: the world as an outside memory. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.

[123]  Edmund T. Rolls,et al.  Neuronal responses in the ventral striatum of the behaving macaque , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[124]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Subcortical connections of inferior temporal areas TE and TEO in macaque monkeys , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[125]  Y. Aloimonos Active Perception , 1993 .

[126]  Pierre Jolicoeur,et al.  Orientation-invariant transfer of training in the identification of rotated natural objects , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[127]  L. Brothers,et al.  Mesial temporal neurons in the macaque monkey with responses selective for aspects of social stimuli , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[128]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[129]  David I. Perrett,et al.  Neurophysiology of shape processing , 1993, Image Vis. Comput..

[130]  David Gaffan,et al.  A Spurious Category-Specific Visual Agnosia for Living Things in Normal Human and Nonhuman Primates , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[131]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Comparison of subcortical connections of inferior temporal and posterior parietal cortex in monkeys , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[132]  TJ Gawne,et al.  How independent are the messages carried by adjacent inferior temporal cortical neurons? , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[133]  M. Young,et al.  An analysis at the population level of the processing of faces in the inferotemporal cortex. , 1993 .

[134]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Evidence for Representations of Perceptually Similar Natural Categories by 3-Month-Old and 4-Month-Old Infants , 1993, Perception.

[135]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  Selectivity for polar, hyperbolic, and Cartesian gratings in macaque visual cortex. , 1993, Science.

[136]  Y. Miyashita Inferior temporal cortex: where visual perception meets memory. , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[137]  C. Gross,et al.  Response properties of neurons in temporal cortical visual areas of infant monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[138]  G. Orban,et al.  Cue-invariant shape selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. , 1993, Science.

[139]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Cortical connections of inferior temporal area TEO in macaque monkeys , 1993, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[140]  I. Biederman,et al.  Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993 .

[141]  P. D. Eimas,et al.  Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants. , 1994, Child development.

[142]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Orientation Invariance and Geometric Primitives in Shape Recognition , 1994, Cogn. Sci..

[143]  H. Sakata,et al.  Parietal control of hand action , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[144]  E. Rolls Brain mechanisms for invariant visual recognition and learning , 1994, Behavioural Processes.

[145]  D. Pandya,et al.  Parietal, temporal, and occipita projections to cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: A retrograde tracer study , 1994, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[146]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala , 1994, Nature.

[147]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal selectivities to complex object features in the ventral visual pathway of the macaque cerebral cortex. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[148]  Nikos K. Logothetis,et al.  Physiological Studies of Motion Inputs. , 1994 .

[149]  P S Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Functional synergism between putative gamma-aminobutyrate-containing neurons and pyramidal neurons in prefrontal cortex. , 1994, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[150]  N. Logothetis,et al.  View-dependent object recognition by monkeys , 1994, Current Biology.

[151]  D. Perrett,et al.  Recognition of objects and their component parts: responses of single units in the temporal cortex of the macaque. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[152]  David I. Perrett,et al.  Modeling visual recognition from neurobiological constraints , 1994, Neural Networks.

[153]  C. Gross,et al.  How inferior temporal cortex became a visual area. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[154]  W. Suzuki,et al.  Topographic organization of the reciprocal connections between the monkey entorhinal cortex and the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[155]  K. Rockland,et al.  Divergent feedback connections from areas V4 and TEO in the macaque , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.

[156]  T. Poggio,et al.  The importance of symmetry and virtual views in three-dimensional object recognition , 1994, Current Biology.

[157]  M. Farah,et al.  The inverted face inversion effect in prosopagnosia: Evidence for mandatory, face-specific perceptual mechanisms , 1995, Vision Research.

[158]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[159]  Minami Ito,et al.  Size and position invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[160]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys , 1995, Current Biology.

[161]  M. Tarr Rotating objects to recognize them: A case study on the role of viewpoint dependency in the recognition of three-dimensional objects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[162]  David C. Knill,et al.  Object classification for human and ideal observers , 1995, Vision Research.

[163]  C. J. Marsolek Abstract visual-form representations in the left cerebral hemisphere. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[164]  M. Bate,et al.  The drosophila neuromuscular junction: a model system for studying synaptic development and function. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[165]  F. J. White,et al.  Synaptic regulation of mesocorticolimbic dopamine neurons. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[166]  G. Yancopoulos,et al.  The neurotrophins and CNTF: two families of collaborative neurotrophic factors. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[167]  R. Wise,et al.  Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[168]  S. Cannon Sodium channel defects in myotonia and periodic paralysis. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[169]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[170]  M. Greenberg,et al.  Intracellular signaling pathways activated by neurotrophic factors. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[171]  N. DeLuca,et al.  Gene transfer to neurons using herpes simplex virus-based vectors. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[172]  R. Menzel,et al.  Learning and memory in honeybees: from behavior to neural substrates. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[173]  M. Farah,et al.  Can recognition of living things be selectively impaired , 1998 .