Age-related and Genetic Modulation of Frontal Cortex Efficiency

The dorsolateral pFC (DLPFC) is a key region for working memory. It has been proposed that the DLPFC is dynamically recruited depending on task demands. By this view, high DLPFC recruitment for low-demanding tasks along with weak DLPFC upregulation at higher task demands reflects low efficiency. Here, the fMRI BOLD signal during working memory maintenance and manipulation was examined in relation to aging and catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158Met status in a large representative sample (n = 287). The efficiency hypothesis predicts a weaker DLPFC response during manipulation, along with a stronger response during maintenance for older adults and COMT Val carriers compared with younger adults and COMT Met carriers. Consistent with the hypothesis, younger adults and met carriers showed maximal DLPFC BOLD response during manipulation, whereas older adults and val carriers displayed elevated DLPFC responses during the less demanding maintenance condition. The observed inverted relations support a link between dopamine and DLPFC efficiency.

[1]  L. Nyberg,et al.  The correlative triad among aging, dopamine, and cognition: Current status and future prospects , 2006, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[2]  Bart Rypma,et al.  Age-dependent relationships between prefrontal cortex activation and processing efficiency , 2011, Cognitive neuroscience.

[3]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  KIBRA Polymorphism Is Related to Enhanced Memory and Elevated Hippocampal Processing , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  John Ashburner,et al.  A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm , 2007, NeuroImage.

[5]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis , 2010, Molecular Psychiatry.

[6]  Alexa M. Morcom,et al.  Neural Reorganization and Compensation in Aging , 2015, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[7]  M Wimber,et al.  Prefrontal dopamine and the dynamic control of human long-term memory , 2011, Translational Psychiatry.

[8]  Lars T Westlye,et al.  Polygenic risk for schizophrenia associated with working memory-related prefrontal brain activation in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. , 2015, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[9]  Lars Bäckman,et al.  Influence of COMT Gene Polymorphism on fMRI-assessed Sustained and Transient Activity during a Working Memory Task , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Irene E. Nagel,et al.  Human Aging Magnifies Genetic Effects on Executive Functioning and Working Memory , 2008, Frontiers in human neuroscience.

[11]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Noise stress impairs prefrontal cortical cognitive function in monkeys: evidence for a hyperdopaminergic mechanism. , 1998, Archives of general psychiatry.

[12]  U. Lindenberger,et al.  Genetics and Functional Imaging: Effects of APOE, BDNF, COMT, and KIBRA in Aging , 2015, Neuropsychology Review.

[13]  R. Dixon,et al.  Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[14]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in human working memory , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  G. Winocur,et al.  The relation between brain activity during memory tasks and years of education in young and older adults. , 2005, Neuropsychology.

[16]  Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  How Does it STAC Up? Revisiting the Scaffolding Theory of Aging and Cognition , 2014, Neuropsychology Review.

[17]  Anna Rieckmann,et al.  Neuromodulation and aging: implications of aging neuronal gain control on cognition , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[18]  A. Meyer-Lindenberg,et al.  Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.

[19]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Neural correlates of variable working memory load across adult age and skill: dissociative patterns within the fronto-parietal network. , 2009, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[20]  J. Duncan An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex , 2001 .

[21]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Maintenance and Manipulation in Working Memory : Differential Ventral and Dorsal Frontal Cortex fMRI Activity , 2009 .

[22]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Inverted-U–Shaped Dopamine Actions on Human Working Memory and Cognitive Control , 2011, Biological Psychiatry.

[23]  Pamela K. Smith,et al.  Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. , 2002, Psychology and aging.

[24]  R. Coppola,et al.  Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. , 2003, Archives of general psychiatry.

[25]  Matti Laine,et al.  Frontal and Temporal Dopamine Release during Working Memory and Attention Tasks in Healthy Humans: a Positron Emission Tomography Study Using the High-Affinity Dopamine D2 Receptor Ligand [11C]FLB 457 , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Andersson,et al.  Longitudinal evidence for diminished frontal cortex function in aging , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  A. Lansner,et al.  Neurocognitive Architecture of Working Memory , 2015, Neuron.

[28]  M. Andersson,et al.  COBRA: A prospective multimodal imaging study of dopamine, brain structure and function, and cognition , 2015, Brain Research.

[29]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Memory aging and brain maintenance , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Computational perspectives on dopamine function in prefrontal cortex , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[31]  L. Nyberg,et al.  The betula prospective cohort study: Memory, health, and aging , 1997 .

[32]  M. Chee,et al.  Functional Imaging of Working Memory after 24 Hr of Total Sleep Deprivation , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  Lars-Göran Nilsson,et al.  COMT Gene Polymorphism Is Associated with Declarative Memory in Adulthood and Old Age , 2004, Behavior genetics.

[34]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[35]  L. Nyberg,et al.  Betula: A Prospective Cohort Study on Memory, Health and Aging , 2004 .

[36]  B. Lipska,et al.  Catechol O-methyltransferase mRNA expression in human and rat brain: evidence for a role in cortical neuronal function , 2003, Neuroscience.

[37]  S. MacDonald,et al.  Simulating Neurocognitive Aging: Effects of a Dopaminergic Antagonist on Brain Activity During Working Memory , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[38]  Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  Age differences in prefontal recruitment during verbal working memory maintenance depend on memory load , 2010, Cortex.

[39]  A. Parkin,et al.  Human memory , 1999, Current Biology.

[40]  Matti Laine,et al.  Effects of Working-Memory Training on Striatal Dopamine Release , 2011, Science.

[41]  S. Folstein,et al.  "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. , 1975, Journal of psychiatric research.

[42]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline , 2011, Nature.